Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Comparison of ‘Mental training’ and physical practice in the mediation of a structured facial examination: a quasi randomized, blinded and controlled study

Abstract

Background

The correct performance of a structured facial examination presents a fundamental clinical skill to detect facial pathologies. However, many students are not adequately prepared in this basic clinical skill. Many argue that the traditional ‘See One, Do One’ approach is not sufficient to fully master a clinical skill. ‘Mental Training’ has successfully been used to train psychomotor and technical skills in sports and other surgical fields, but its use in Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery is not described. We conducted a quasi-experimental to determine if ‘Mental Training’ was effective in teaching a structured facial examination.

Methods

Sixty-seven students were randomly assigned to a ‘Mental Training’ and ‘See One, Do One’ group. Both groups received standardized video instruction on how to perform a structured facial examination. The ‘See One, Do One’ group then received 60 min of guided physical practice while the ‘Mental Training’ group actively developed a detailed, stepwise sequence of the performance of a structured facial examination and visualized this sequence subvocally before practicing the skill. Student performance was measured shortly after (T1) and five to 10 weeks (T2) after the training by two blinded examiners (E1 and E2) using a validated checklist.

Results

Groups did not differ in gender, age or in experience. The ‘Mental Training’ group averaged significantly more points in T1 (pE1 = 0.00012; pE2 = 0.004; dE1 = 0.86; dE2 = 0.66) and T2 (pE1 = 0.04; pE2 = 0.008, dE1 = 0.37; dE2 = 0.64) than the ‘See One, Do One’ group. The intragroup comparison showed a significant (pE1 = 0.0002; pE2 = 0.06, dE1 = 1.07; dE2 = 0.50) increase in clinical examination skills in the ‘See One, Do One’ group, while the ‘Mental Training’ group maintained an already high level of clinical examination skills between T1 and T2.

Discussion

‘Mental Training’ is an efficient tool to teach and maintain basic clinical skills. In this study ‘Mental Training’ was shown to be superior to the commonly used ‘See One, Do One’ approach in learning how to perform a structured facial examination and should therefore be considered more often to teach physical examination skills.

Peer Review reports

Background

Practical skills must be learned by every medical student regardless of her or his future specialization [1]. However, studies show that students consider the training of practical skills in the context of their medical education as insufficient [2] and there are still shortcomings in the training of basic clinical skills such as the physical examination in undergraduate medical education [3,4,5]. Although, many authors have demonstrated deficits in the training of clinical skills, the methodological approaches in teaching methods to impart competencies in those skills are not completely elaborate. Therefore, it is necessary to find effective educational strategies for each clinical skill.

In medical education, clinical skills are traditionally taught using the ‘See One, Do One’ method. In 1889, Halsted introduced a system in which medical students completed a university-sponsored, hospital-based surgical training program [6]. Halsted’s model of ‘See One, Do One, Teach One’ is based on acquiring increasing amounts of responsibility that culminate in near independence. Halsted was not only interested in developing a system to train surgeons, but also in creating teachers and role models [7]. Today this approach is labelled as the main component of clinical-bedside teaching. Students learn by watching an expert explaining and demonstrating a skill. This is followed by the first independent performance of the skill, which is mostly with a patient [7]. Although this method has been obviously proven to be effective, reality is forcing us to establish new teaching methods. The enormous workload including reduced lengths of stay in hospital combined with increasing case numbers, growing multimorbidity and increased complexity of treatments leads to significantly reduced chances for bedside teaching [8]. Often, the important last step ‘Teach one’ is often omitted or is undertaken without supervision in everyday clinical practice. Further, the health care system is facing increased economic pressure and scarcity of resources [9].

Thus, it would be optimal to establish a teaching method that does not involve many resources. Recently there has been a growing understanding that cognitive abilities, such as problem solving and movement-planning, play a crucial role in learning practical skills [10, 11]. This has resulted in a shift away from training methods that exclusively focus on the acquisition of motor skills but rather target the actual thought process when performing a clinical skill. This way of cognitive or mentally training in a certain skill has long been established in sports [12, 13] and has also been shown to be beneficial in the retrieval of motor abilities in rehabilitative medicine [14]. A reason discussed for its effectiveness resides in Janerod’s simulation theory [15] which hypotheses that the motor system is also part of a bigger cognitive network which includes various psychological activities. During this ‘Mental Training’, similar neural pathways are activated and similar changes in the brain take place as when actually performing a motor skill as functional MRI investigations have shown [15, 16]. This explains why ‘Mental Training’ can directly improve motor skills and induce a similar neuroplasticity as physical practice and thus can help to close the gap between the observation and the execution of a skill [17].

‘Mental Training’ has already been used successfully in the mediation of clinical skills and surgical training [17,18,19,20]. An influential study by Arora et al. [21] showed that participants who practiced with a standardized mental imaginary script showed greater improvements in learning laparoscopic cholecystectomies compared to a control group that participated in online lecture training. These results were supported by Immenroth et al., who demonstrated that mental rehearsal led to significantly better performance results compared to practical training in the mediation of laparoscopic cystectomies [19]. Despite these results, not all studies report a beneficial effect from ‘Mental Training’. Studies by Jungmann et al. and Sanders et al., for example, found no significant effect of ‘Mental Training’ compared to physical practice in the acquisition of laparoscopic knot-tying and basic surgical skills [22, 23].

In 2015, Rao et al. published a meta-analysis investigating ‘Mental Training’ in medical under- and postgraduate education for the training of practical skills [24]. Their analysis showed a great heterogeneity regarding the chosen methodology and design of the training process and also the skills that had to be learned widely differed. Furthermore, it is unclear if ‘Mental Training’ really presents an advantage in the training of clinical skills compared to traditional teaching methods.

Therefore, the aim of this study was to prospectively investigate the teaching efficacy of two teaching methods, namely a ‘Mental Training’ approach (study group) and the traditional ‘See One, Do One’ approach (control group) in the short- and long-term acquisition of a basic clinical skill in the field of Oral- and Maxillofacial Surgery, namely the structured facial examination of the head and face. Another aim of this study was to investigate the curricular (‘in vivo’) feasibility of the ‘Mental Training’ approach. The hypothesis of this study was that ‘Mental Training’ would be equal compared to the ‘See One, Do One’ approach.

Methods

Study design and participants

Overall, 67 (female n = 49; male n = 18) 4th year dentistry students on a five-year program without previous experience in the field of Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery were assigned quasi-randomly to a ‘Mental Training’ group and a ‘See One, Do One’ group which was regarded as a control (Fig. 1). Participation in the study was voluntary and took place after written informed consent, which was revocable at any time. Students were blinded in relation to their knowledge of the didactic principles used during their training as well as affiliation to any study group. Basic data regarding student age, sex, and duration of study were collected using a questionnaire.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study design and execution

The study was conducted according to the ethical principles of the Helsinki Declaration (Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects), and the local ethics committee noted that no further approval was necessary.

Assignment of the students to the instructional approaches

The assignment of students to one of the learning groups with a maximum of 6 students per week occurred prior to the Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery apprenticeship independent of the authors and independent of study participation by the dean’s office. Groups were then assigned alternately within the 10-week span of the apprenticeship to one of the instructional approaches. This quasi-randomized allocation of students was chosen due to the curricular ‘in-vivo’ study design and intended to produce similar groups.

Study protocol

The study was carried out within the Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery apprenticeship for dentistry students, which includes a five-day rotation through every section of the Department of Oral, Cranio-Maxillofacial and Facial Plastic Surgery, i.e. the operative room, the outpatient clinic or the emergency department. Before starting their rotation, students have to complete a practical skills training [25]. The aim is to give dentistry students a short overview of the most common reasons for consultation in Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery and prepare them for the upcoming clinic rotation. It is divided into a theoretical part (240 min) in the morning and practical skills training (240 min), during which the study took place, in the afternoon. Trained practical skills include performing a structured facial examination, placing a venous catheter in the context of an emergency treatment and placing an ‘Ernst ligature’ on a phantom model. Lessons were held in small groups ranging from five to six students.

‘See One, Do One’ approach

As quality assurance and standardization for the demonstration of a structured facial examination, the skill was videotaped based on the existing manual and checklist. The trainer demonstrated the video and explained the performance of a structured facial examination step-by step in detail. The explanations were predetermined in the manual and trained in the tutor training, which was identical for both approaches. Subsequently, students practiced the skill on each other under supervision and, if needed, correction of the tutor. Each student was advised to perform the structured facial examination at least once. In total the training lasted for 60 min.

‘Mental training’ approach

For the ‘Mental Training’ approach, the students received the same standardized video instruction of a structured facial examination. Subsequently, students practiced the skill under the supervision and, if needed, correction of the tutor for 30 min. Students then actively developed an individual detailed, stepwise sequence of the performance of a structured facial examination under the supervision and review of the tutor. Students were free to divide the skill into individual sub-steps which they noted on index cards. This process of creating an individual ‘mental map’ was supervised by tutor who would integrate missing steps or correct the sequence, if necessary. Students then visualized and internalized every step of the individual mental map in subvocal for 30 min. In total the training lasted for 60 min.

Performance measurement

In order to assess the acquired competence in performing a structured facial examination, the OSCE-format was used during the training week directly after the intervention in the last 60 min of the practical skills training (T1) as a single station OSCE and 5 to 10 weeks later (T2) as part of a curricular and formative Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery OSCE (8 stations in total). A three point scale scale was used (0 points for not done, 1 point for done, but incorrect, 2 points for done and correct) for the checklist, which was based on the checklist used in the tutor manual (Supplement 1). In total, the checklist consisted of 24 items, which equals a maximum score of 48 points. The checklists implemented had been primarily piloted in previous undergraduate trainings that were not related to the mental training approach. In addition, the content validity was ensured through its creation as part of an expert workshop with didactic and surgical experts as well as through repeated application and adaption in the context of previous studies [25, 26] and OSCE exams. A timeframe of 5 min to complete each OSCE station was given.

Both at T1 and T2, students were video-recorded (Camera System: Panasonic HC-X929) for later performance measurement by two independent, blinded examiners with different levels of experience (Rater 1: Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery resident in year four of a five-year training; Rater 2: Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery resident in year three of a five-year training).

Data analysis

Microsoft Office 2016 (Microsoft Office 2007,© Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, USA) for Mac and SPSS Statistics version 19 (IBM, Armonk, USA) were used for the statistical analysis and graphical display of data.

To test for a normal distribution of the data, the Shapiro-Wilks-Test was used. Since the test results for both groups were not normally distributed at all times, the Mann-Whitney-U-Test for non-parametric data was used to test for significant differences in learning success in the inter- and intragroup comparison at T1 and T2. Furthermore, effect sizes were calculated for T1 to T2 using Cohens d. Cohen’s d is defined as the difference between two means divided by a standard deviation for the data resulting in an unitless value that helps to interpret the effect size of observed results and hence the statistical power of a study. For most types of effect sizes, a larger absolute value indicates a stronger effect. Since the sample size (n = 67) of our study was relatively small, Cohen’s d was used as an additional control test since prior studies have shown significant test results alone are not sufficient to interpret data and draw conclusions from this data [27]. The inter-rater reliability was measured using Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r).

Since there was a gender imbalance between female and male study participants, a gender analysis was conducted to investigate the effect of this imbalance on the outcome of the different tests, comparing the results of all female and male study participants.

Sample size estimation

Based on prior examination results from the years before the intervention, we estimated an average student performance of 70% with a standard deviation of 10% in the OSCE. Based on the following parameters (Mean ‘Mental Training’ = 33, Mean ‘See One, Do One’ = 30, SD = 5, alpha = 0.05, beta = 0.2) a sample size of 88 was calculated.

Results

Study participants

Overall, 67 (f = 49; m = 18) students agreed to participate in the study. A total of 29 students (m = 8, w = 21) belonged to the ‘See One, Do One’ group while 38 students (m = 11, w = 29) were trained using the ‘Mental Training’ approach. Students from the ‘See One, Do One’ group (average age = 25,2, average duration of study = 8,5 semesters) did not significantly differ in baseline characteristics compared to the ‘Mental Training’ group (average age = 24,8, average duration of study = 8,5 semesters). The gender distribution corresponded the gender distribution at Goethe University’s Dental School [27]. All students (f = 49; m = 18) participated in the curricular OSCE 5–10 weeks after the intervention. Both teaching interventions could be carried out in a curricular setting with a 100% participation rate in the given timeframe without any complications.

Outcome measures

At T1, students who had been trained with the ‘Mental Training’ approach showed highly significant better results in the performance of a structured facial examination (Rater 1 p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.86; Rater 2 p = 0.005; Cohen’s d = 0.66, r = 0.74) than students who had been trained with the ‘See One, Do One’ approach (Table 1). In the long-term retention test 5–10 weeks after the intervention (T2), students who had been trained with the ‘Mental Training’ approach again showed significantly better results than students who had been trained with the ‘See One, Do One’ approach (Rater 1 p < 0.05; Cohen’s d = 0.38; Rater 2 p = 0.008; Cohen’s d = 0.64, r = 0.64) (Table 1). The intragroup comparison between T1 and T2 revealed a significant increase in clinical examination skills in the ‘See One, Do One’ group (Rater 1 p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 1.07; Rater 2 p = 0.06; Cohen’s d = 0.50) (Table 1). The ‘Mental Training’ group did not show a significant growth in clinical examination skills from T1 to T2 (Rater 1 p < 0.10; Cohen’s d = 0.35; Rater 2 p = 0.06; Cohen’s d = 0.63) (Table 1). However, this group was able to maintain a very high level of competence from T1 to T2 (Table 1).

Table 1 Average scores obtained by the ‘Mental Training’ (MT) and ‘See One, Do One’ (SODO) group in the OSCE (48 possible points) and corresponding significance levels. Data are presented as Mean ± SD

Gender analysis

Overall, no significant differences were found between female and male students at T1 and T2. Only Rater 1 found a significant difference at T1 between both genders (Table 2).

Table 2 Average scores obtained by female and male students in the OSCE (48 possible points) and corresponding significance levels. Data are presented as Mean ± SD

Discussion

The correct performance of a structured facial examination represents a fundamental basic clinical skill that is of great importance for the ongoing physician because of the high frequency of craniofacial trauma (48.1% of all injuries with an Abbreviated Injury Scale > 3) [28, 29] especially since previous studies have shown significant shortcomings regarding Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery-related knowledge and skills in undergraduate education [30,31,32]. The aim of this study was to prospectively investigate the teaching efficacy of two teaching methods, namely a ‘Mental Training’ approach (study group) and the traditional ‘See One, Do One’ approach (control group) in the short- and long-term acquisition of the above-mentioned basic clinical skill. Another aim of this study was to investigate the curricular (‘in vivo’) feasibility of the ‘Mental Training’ approach.

Overall, our results revealed significant performance differences between both groups in the short-term examination (T1) in favor of the ‘Mental Training’ group. Furthermore, the examination 5–10 weeks later (T2) revealed a significantly better long-term learning retention of the acquired practical skills for this group. Students in the control group, however, had also significantly improved their level of competence by the long-term comparison. The implementation of the ‘Mental Training’ approach in a curricular setting was completely feasible within the given timeframe for the Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery apprenticeship for dentistry students.

Interestingly, the control group had significantly improved its performance by the long-term examination at T2 while the study group managed to maintain a high performance level. The performance assessment at T2 as part of a curricular and formative surgical OSCE might have led to led to the significant performance improvement of the control-group in the long-term comparison measured. Raupach et al. described this phenomenon before and found that an examination itself can significantly enhance student performance independent of the chosen instructional approach [33].

We believe a possible reason for the found performance differences in favor of the ‘Mental Training’ approach in the short- and long-term measurement is the active visualization and verbalization in subvocal of the previously defined steps for a structured facial examination. The active imagination of the performance of a complex motor skill might be a reason for the better performance by the study group since students were forced to cognitively process and memorize each step of the skill before performing it. Previous studies came to similar conclusions [15,16,17]. For example, Janerod et al. hypothesized in their simulation theory that the motor system is part of a bigger cognitive network which includes various psychological activities [15]. They concluded that during mental training for a motor skill, similar neural pathway are activated and similar changes in the brain take place as when actually performing a motor skill. This hypothesis has been supported by functional MRI investigations. For example Seiler et al. could show similar brain activation patterns during arm rotation tasks while mentally training them [16]. This could explain why ‘Mental Training’ led to a significant improvement in our study group and was even shown to be superior compared to the control group regarding the acquisition of a basic clinical skill since it not only induced a similar neuroplasticity as physical practice in the ‘See One, Do One’ approach but may also have helped to close the gap between the observation and the execution of a complex skill [17].

There is a great heterogeneity regarding the impact of ‘Mental Training’ in surgical education in the current literature. There are randomized and controlled studies that have shown that Mental Training led to significantly better performance results in the mediation of laparoscopic cystectomies [19, 21]. However, not all studies that investigated ‘Mental Training’ in surgical education reported beneficial effects. In the mediation of laparoscopic knot-tying or surgical suturing and knot-tying no significant effect of ‘Mental Training’ compared to physical practice could be found [22, 23]. A possible explanation for this heterogeneity of outcome measures regarding ‘Mental Training’ might be the degree of complexity of a certain trained motor skill. Compared to the performance of a structured facial examination or the performance of a laparoscopic cystectomy, there are fewer steps when performing a simple surgical suture which also leaves less room for mistakes. This difference in complexity might explain why ‘Mental Training’ is especially suitable for more complex motor skills. Future studies have to investigate for what degree of complexity and which motor skills in particular mental training is superior compared to physical practice or other approaches like the use of teaching associates [34].

Overall, we could show that ‘Mental Training’ is significantly more effective than the traditional ‘See One, Do One’ approach in training for a structured facial examination and also led to significantly better knowledge retention in the long-term. Moreover, we demonstrated a ‘Mental Training’ protocol that was feasible in a curricular ‘in vivo’ setting with a given timeframe for teaching small groups.

Limitations and strengths

Due to the limited curricular timeframe an objective assessment prior to the intervention could not be carried out which can be viewed as shortcoming of our study. However, both groups were naïve in terms of their knowledge on how to perform a structured facial examination and did not significantly differ in gender, age or academic success, which indicates that they were equivalent prior to the intervention. The sample size (n = 67 students) might be another limitation to the statistical power of the study.

However, compared to other studies, the present study was quasi-randomized, controlled, blinded, carried out within a curricular framework and assessed student performance over a six- to ten-week span. Moreover, it included an entire cross-section of an 8th semester at an accredited dental school. Future studies have to investigate whether our results can be transferred to other subjects and faculties.

Conclusion

To our knowledge, this study is the first to have compared ‘Mental Training’ with the often used ‘See One, Do One’ approach in the mediation of a structured facial examination within a curricular framework.

‘Mental Training’ is significantly more effective than the ‘See One, Do one’ approach in the mediation of a structured facial examination in the short-and long-term and its implementation within a larger scale of students was completely possible.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

OSCE:

Objective Structured Clinical Examination

References

  1. Goodwin J. The importance of clinical skills. BMJ. 1995;310(6990):1281–2. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmj.310.6990.1281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Kaur N, Gupta A, Saini P. A needs assessment study of undergraduate surgical education. Natl Med J India. 2011;24:292–3.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Goodfellow PB, Claydon P. Students Sitting Medical Finals–-Ready to be House Officers? J R Soc Med. 2001;94:516–20 [cited 2020 Mar 26] Available from: http://0-journals-sagepub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1177/014107680109401007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Ladak A, Hanson J, de Gara CJ. What procedures are students doing during undergraduate surgical clerkship? Can J Surg. 2006;49:329–34 [cited 2020 mar 26] Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/17152570.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Liddell MJ, Davidson SK, Taub H, Whitecross LE. Evaluation of procedural skills training in an undergraduate curriculum. Med Educ. 2002;36:1035–41 [cited 2020 Mar 26] Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1046/j.1365-2923.2002.01306.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Cameron JL. William Stewart Halsted: Our surgical heritage. Ann Surg. 1997;225:445–58.

  7. Kotsis SV, Chung KC. Application of the &quot;see one, do one, teach one&quot; concept in surgical training. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2013;131:1194–201 [cited 2018 Oct 11] Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/23629100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Marburger Bund Monitoring 2017. [cited 2017 Nov 20]. Available from: https://www.marburger-bund.de/der-marburger-bund/projekte-und-kampagnen/mitgliederbefragung/2017

  9. Frenk J, Chen L, Bhutta ZA, Cohen J, Crisp N, Evans T, et al. Health professionals for a new century: Ttransforming education to strengthen health systems in an interdependent world. Lancet; 2010. p. 1923–1958.

  10. Kahol K, Smith M, Mayes S, Deka M, Deka V, Ferrara J, et al. The effect of fatigue on cognitive and psychomotor skills of surgical residents. Lect Notes Comput Sci (including Subser Lect Notes Artif Intell Lect Notes Bioinformatics). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg; 2007. p. 304–13.

  11. Yule S, Flin R, Paterson-Brown S, Maran N, Rowley D. Development of a rating system for surgeons’ non-technical skills. Med Educ. 2006;40:1098–104 [cited 2020 Mar 26] Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1365-2929.2006.02610.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Sevdalis N, Moran A, Arora S. Mental imagery and mental practice applications in surgery: State of the art and future directions. Multisensory Imag. Springer New York; 2013. p. 343–63.

  13. Wingfield LR, Kulendran M, Chow A, Nehme J, Purkayastha S. Cognitive Task Analysis: Bringing Olympic Athlete Style Training to Surgical Education. Surg Innov. 2015;22:406–17 [cited 2020 Mar 26] Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/25392150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Cicerone KD, Langenbahn DM, Braden C, Malec JF, Kalmar K, Fraas M, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: Updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil; 2011. p. 519–530.

  15. Schinke R, McGannon K, Smith B. Routledge international handbook of sport psychology. 2016 [cited 2020 Mar 26]. Available from: https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=MneFCwAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PP1&dq=Moran+A.+Expertise+and+mental+practice.+In:+Schinke+RJ,+McGannon+KR,+Smith+B,+editors.+The+Routledge+international+handbook+of+sport+psychology.+Routledge.+13.&ots=bFio6t3nTi&sig=t0-eShQpcGZF_V5PCCbsmdzT3Es.

  16. Seiler BD, Monsma EV, Newman-Norlund RD. Biological evidence of imagery abilities: Intraindividual differences. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2015;37:421–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Eberspächer H, Immenroth M. [Mental training--does it also help the modern surgeon?]. Zentralbl Chir. 1999;124:895–901. [cited 2020 Mar 26] Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/10596047.

  18. Mayer J, Bohn J, Görlich P, Eberspächer H. Mentales Gehtraining - Wirksamkeit eines Therapieverfahrens in der Rehabilitation nach Hüftendoprothetik. Z Orthop Ihre Grenzgeb. 2005;143:419–23 [cited 2020 Mar 26] Available from: http://0-www-thieme--connect-de.brum.beds.ac.uk/DOI/DOI?10.1055/s-2005-836829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Immenroth M, Bürger T, Brenner J, Nagelschmidt M, Eberspächer H, Troidl H. Mental training in surgical education: A randomized controlled trial. Ann Surg. 2007;245:385–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Komesu Y, Urwitz-Lane R, Ozel B, Lukban J, Kahn M, Muir T, et al. Does mental imagery prior to cystoscopy make a difference? A randomized controlled trial. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2009;201:218 e1–218.e9. [cited 2018 Jul 13] Available from: http://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S0002937809003949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Arora S, Aggarwal R, Sirimanna P, Moran A, Grantcharov T, Kneebone R, et al. Mental Practice Enhances Surgical Technical Skills. Ann Surg. 2011;253:265–70 [cited 2018 Jul 13] Available from: http://0-insights-ovid-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/crossref?an=00000658-201102000-00009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Jungmann F, Gockel I, Hecht H, Kuhr K, Räsänen J, Sihvo E, et al. Impact of Perceptual Ability and Mental Imagery Training on Simulated Laparoscopic Knot-Tying in Surgical Novices Using a Nissen Fundoplication Model. Scand J Surg. 2011;100:78–85 [cited 2018 Jul 13] Available from: http://0-journals-sagepub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1177/145749691110000203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Sanders CW, Sadoski M, van Walsum K, Bramson R, Wiprud R, Fossum TW. Learning basic surgical skills with mental imagery: using the simulation centre in the mind. Med Educ. 2008;42:607–12 [cited 2018 Jul 13] Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2007.02964.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Rao A, Tait I, Alijani A. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the role of mental training in the acquisition of technical skills in surgery. Am J Surg. 2015;210:545–53 [cited 2018 Jul 13] Available from: http://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S0002961015002263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Landes CA, Hoefer S, Schuebel F, Ballon A, Teiler A, Tran A, Weber R, Walcher F, Sader R. Long-term prospective teaching effectivity of practical skills training and a first OSCE in cranio maxillofacial surgery for dental students. J Craniomaxillofac Surg. 2014;42(5):e97–104. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jcms.2013.07.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Höfer SH, Schuebel F, Sader R, Landes C. Development and implementation of an objective structured clinical examination (OSCE) in CMF-surgery for dental students. J Craniomaxillofac Surg. 2013;41(5):412–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jcms.2012.11.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. STUDIERENDENSTATISTIK Sommersemester 2020. Available from: www.statkap.uni-frankfurt.de

  28. Moses H, Powers D, Keeler J, Erdmann D, Marcus J, Puscas L, et al. Opportunity Cost of Surgical Management of Craniomaxillofacial Trauma. Craniomaxillofacial Trauma Reconstr. 2015;09:076–81. [cited 2017 Jul 31] Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/26889352.

  29. Traumaregister DGU. 2017 [cited 2017 Nov 7]; Available from: http://www.traumaregister-dgu.de/fileadmin/user_upload/traumaregister-dgu.de/docs/Downloads/TR-DGU-Jahresbericht_2017.pdf

  30. Seifert LB, Hoefer SH, Flammiger S, Rüsseler M, Thieringer F, Ehrenfeld M, et al. A nationwide survey of undergraduate training in oral and maxillofacial surgery. Oral Maxillofac Surg. 2018;1–8. [cited 2018 May 25] Available from: http://0-link-springer-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s10006-018-0703-0

  31. Seifert LB, Sterz J, Bender B, Sader R, Ruesseler M, Hoefer SH. Undergraduate medical students need more training in craniomaxillofacial surgery: a comparative study between medical and dental students. Innov Surg Sci. 2017 [cited 2017 Nov 7]; Available from: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Lukas_Seifert2/publication/318881199_Undergraduate_medical_students_need_more_training_in_craniomaxillofacial_surgery_a_comparative_study_between_medical_and_dental_students/links/5988949daca27266ada4d600/Undergraduate

  32. Ologunde R, Sykes M. A review of oral and maxillofacial surgery journals’ contribution to undergraduate surgical education. Br J Oral Maxillofac Surg. 2014;52:894–900 [cited 2017 Mar 23] Available from: http://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S0266435614005609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Raupach T, Brown J, Anders S, Hasenfuss G, Harendza S. Summative assessments are more powerful drivers of student learning than resource intensive teaching formats. BMC Med. 2013;11:61 [cited 2017 Nov 26] Available from: http://0-bmcmedicine-biomedcentral-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/articles/10.1186/1741-7015-11-61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Hoefer SH, Sterz J, Bender B, Stefanescu M-C, Theis M, Walcher F, et al. Conveying practical clinical skills with the help of teaching associates—a randomised trial with focus on the long term learning retention. BMC Med Educ. 2017;17:65 [cited 2018 Apr 11] Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/28351359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all students that participated in the study.

Funding

This study was not funded. Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All Authors (AN, BS, AG, JS, RS, MR and LS) had a relevant contribution to the manuscript and have read and approved the final manuscript. LS was responsible for study conception and design. AN performed the data collection and statistical analysis. All authors read, edited and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lukas B. Seifert.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved as a program evaluation by the Ethical Commission of the University Hospital Frankfurt (Goethe University) since it didn’t involve biomedical research within the meaning of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants of the study gave their written consent for participation, which they could withdraw at any time.

Consent for publication

Not applicable since no individual person’s data was included in the manuscript.

Competing interests

All authors declare that they have no financial or non-financial competing interests that might create a conflict of interest with the information presented in this article. One of the authors (MR) is also a member of BMC Medical Education’s editorial board.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplement 1.

OSCE Checklist – Structured Facial Examination.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nelskamp, A., Schnurr, B., Germanyuk, A. et al. Comparison of ‘Mental training’ and physical practice in the mediation of a structured facial examination: a quasi randomized, blinded and controlled study. BMC Med Educ 21, 178 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-021-02603-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-021-02603-0