Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

The impact of writing on academic performance for medical students

Abstract

Background

Writing is a useful learning activity that promotes higher-order thinking, but there are limited studies that prove its effectiveness. In previous research, researchers tested the effect of summary writing on students’ comprehension and found no significant difference from that of re-studying texts. The purpose of this study, therefore, is to expand previous findings and investigate the effect of two types of writing tasks on medical students’ academic performance, specifically in the transfer of knowledge.

Methods

An experiment was conducted with 139 medical students from Seoul National University College of Medicine. They were randomly assigned to three study conditions: self-study (SS), expository writing (EW), and argumentative writing (AW) group. Each group studied the given material by the method they were assigned, and they were tested on their comprehension and transfer of knowledge using rote-memory type items and transfer type items respectively.

Results

The results showed that the two writing groups displayed better performance than the SS group in transfer type items, while there was no difference in scores between the EW and AW group. However, the three groups showed no significant difference in their scores for rote-memory type items. Also, there was a positive correlation between the writing scores and transfer type item scores in the AW group.

Conclusions

This study provides empirical evidence for writing to be adopted in medical education for greater educational benefits. Our findings indicate that writing can enhance learning and higher-order thinking, which are critical for medical students.

Peer Review reports

Background

“Writing organizes and clarifies our thoughts. Writing is how we think our way into a subject and make it our own. Writing enables us to find out what we know—and what we don’t know—about whatever we’re trying to learn.” [1] As Zinsser once stated, we can clarify what we know and what we do not know through writing. The process of writing requires writers to have a clear understanding of the subject matter [2] and make use of cognitive abilities. Specifically, writing helps students develop higher-order thinking skills that involve three cognitive processes - analysis, evaluation, and creation [3]. At the beginning of the writing process, students should first create a sound argument and express it clearly. Afterward, they should constantly analyze and evaluate what has been written to make a persuasive claim without logical fallacies.

These higher-order thinking skills are needed for medical students to grow as successful medical professionals [4]. Doctors need to diagnose the problem of a patient, mentally represent the situation, plan appropriate treatments, and evaluate the whole process to check against other possibilities [5,6,7]. With limited research regarding the learning effects of writing in medical education, some medical schools make use of writing in their classrooms. By way of illustration, students of Maastricht College of Medicine are required to submit a portfolio that includes reflection papers on the roles and abilities of medical professionals, scientists, and health care providers, respectively [8]. Since these essays are written multiple times over semesters or years, students have the opportunity to reflect upon their whole learning process and look back on what they have learned.

While writing should be emphasized to foster higher-order thinking skills for future medical professionals, previous literature reports conflicting evidence on the effect of writing [9,10,11]. Representatively, Spirgel and Delaney (2016) concluded that summary writing was not effective than re-studying learning materials [12]. They also found that students better remembered only the items that were included in the summary compared to those that were omitted. However, it is early to conclude that students cannot learn enough from practicing writing, because in the previous research, they only measured students’ comprehension using rote-memory test items without testing the transfer of knowledge. In the end, applying learned knowledge to different areas is as important as understanding difficult concepts to deal with new patients and novel situations every day. When learners apply information, strategies, and skills they have learned to new contexts, transfer of knowledge occurs [13]. In this context, while summary writing may not have been more effective in memorizing learned contents [9], there is evidence that writing facilitates the transfer of knowledge. For example, Boscolo and Mason (2001) found that learning historical events through writing could be transferred to learning concepts in a different domain, science [14]. Considering that the ultimate goal of education should be to improve thinking skills and application of knowledge, the effects of writing on learning should also be measured in the context of the transfer of knowledge along with accurate memorization [15, 16].

Besides, different types of writing could have different effects on students’ learning because each writing task focuses on different skill sets. For instance, summary writing requires skills such as classification, comparison, definition, and illustration; it requires a structured interpretation of the texts and thus can be more effective in improving comprehension [17]. On the other hand, through argumentation, students can engage in a deeper and more mature level of learning [18]. When writing an argumentative essay writers need to establish contested claims while formulating an explanation, generating counterarguments, and assessing them to support their own opinions [3]. While both types of writing can help students develop thinking skills, writing an argumentative essay involves more reasoning [19] and promotes critical thinking, like analyzing and synthesizing an argument [20]. All in all, depending on which type of task teachers use, writing can be more effective in stimulating the transfer of knowledge and higher-order thinking beyond comprehension.

Thus, in this study, we investigated the effects of writing on the transfer of knowledge in a medical education setting. If there is empirical evidence that writing can help learn factual knowledge and enhance higher-order thinking skills at the same time, writing can then be used to complement traditional methods of learning to foster medical professionals who have broader perspectives. We developed the following study design: medical students were first divided into three studying conditions, those who study by themselves (self-study: SS), those who study by writing a summary text (summary writing: SW), and those who study by writing an argumentative essay (argumentative writing: AW). Then, we measured students’ academic performance through a final test that assessed both rote-memory and transfer of knowledge. Our hypotheses are as follows: First, all groups (SS, SW, and AW) will show no significant difference in their performance in rote-memory type items. Second, students in the writing groups (SW and AW) will score higher in transfer-type items; specifically, students who study by writing an argumentative essay (AW) will perform better than those who write a summary text (SW).

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited at the Seoul National University College of Medicine. Among 139 individuals, 48 were female. Twenty-three participants who failed to follow the directions were excluded from the study. Therefore, only the data from the remaining 116 participants were analyzed (Mage = 19.22, SDage = 0.79).

Material

The participants were instructed to study 4-page-long written material. The study methods varied depending on their assigned groups. The subject matter dealt with the relationship between youth’s cognitive development and musical skills. This subject was chosen because it is less likely to be affected by background knowledge since related courses are not provided to the medical school students. Also, it was convenient to devise final test questions and the writing tasks based on the topic as the material covered diverse concepts and theories. We were also able to refer to an already existing set of test questions on this topic, verified and used in the National Teacher Certification Examination in Korea.

Experiment procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to either of the three groups: the SS (self-study) group, the SW (summary writing) group, and the AW (argumentative writing) group. For the SS group, participants were instructed to study the written material by themselves for 25 min. For the writing groups, participants were instructed to write a half-page long essay on the given material for 25 min. The SW group was instructed to summarize the given text, while the AW group was instructed to create an argument based on what they learned. Specifically, the SW group was told to write more than three paragraphs, the total length of over a half-page. The group had to summarize various stages in youth’s cognitive development of music, which was mainly handled in the learning material. On the other hand, the AW group was instructed to write an argumentative essay. The length of the writing required was identical to the SW group. The task required the participants to pretend they were elementary school music teachers and propose a music class based on the cognitive development theories introduced in the material. Four developmental theorists of music were suggested by Zimmerman, Hargreaves, Gardner, and Swanwick & Tillman, each theory having distinctive views [21]. Students had to choose one of the theories and argue what they chose was better than the others. Following the study session, participants were asked to solve the Remote Associates Test, which also served as a filler task, for 15 min. Finally, they were given 20 min to complete a final test on the learning material.

Remote associates test (RAT)

Remote Associates Test (RAT) is a test commonly used to assesses creativity [22]. This goes in context with one of the cognitive processes of higher-order thinking- creation or creative thinking [23]. To see how creative thinking is related to writing and final performance, fifteen questions were selected and used from the question pool published by Mednick [24, 25]. The student being tested has to think of a fourth word that is somehow related to the three words given and all questions have one definite answer.

Measurement of academic performance

Final test questions were comprised of the rote-memory type and transfer type items. The ten rote-memory type items asked direct factual information on the given material and were worth 13 points. There were four transfer type items, which required the students to think a step further and apply what they learned to new situations. These items required not only an overall comprehension of the given material but also an application of it to different situations, which were worth 16 points. Thus, the maximum score of students could achieve was 29 points. Although most of the questions required the participants to write a short or narrative answer, these items had definite answers and guidelines for evaluating students’ performance. Furthermore, to rule out any subjective evaluation by the experimenters, three raters’ agreement for the scores on transfer type items was measured using Intraclass correlation. The coefficient value showed high agreement among the three raters (ICC (3, k) = .930).

Writing scores

To measure participants’ writing performance, we scored their essays based on the criteria proposed by Lumley [26]. Specifically, we scored students’ writing on three criteria. First, we rated whether the writing was cohesive and organized (3 points). Second, whether it was appropriate to the task and given materials (2 points), in addition to whether the writing was easily comprehendible (2 points). Third, whether all aspects of presentation conventions including spelling, punctuation, and structure are handled skillfully (3 points). In total, the maximum writing score was 10 points.

Statistical analysis

To examine the effect of different study conditions on academic performance, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), linear regression analysis, and correlation analysis were performed. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 23 software (SPSS, Chicago, L, USA) and R (3.6.2. version; R Foundation, Vienna, Austria). The statistical significance for all tests was set as α < 0.05.

Results

To begin with, ANCOVA was conducted to compare the academic performance of the students in the three groups. To summarize the results shown in Table 1, there was a significant difference in total test scores between the three groups (P = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.102). The difference in scores for transfer type items was also significant (P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.249). However, no difference was found in the scores for rote-memory type items (P = 0.899, ηp2 = 0.002).

Table 1 Academic performance by group and type of final test items

Analyzing the differences in more detail, the two writing groups showed significantly higher total test scores than the SS group (P = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.064). The SS group scored significantly lower than the SW group (13.34 vs. 17.11, P = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.077); however, no significant difference was found between the SS group and the AW group (13.34 vs. 16.54, P = 0.109, ηp2 = 0.023). The average total score of the AW group was not significantly different from that of the SW group (16.54 vs. 17.11, P = 0.197, ηp2 = 0.015).

For rote-memory type items, there was no significant difference between the three groups. The performance of the writing groups was not significantly higher than that of the SS group (P = 0.937, ηp2 = 0.000). To re-emphasize, the score of the SS group for rote-memory type items was not significantly different from those of the SW group or the AW group (9.26 vs. 9.14, P = 0.858, ηp2 = 0.000; 9.26 vs. 9.57, P = 0.756, ηp2 = 0.001). The performance of the AW group was not significantly different from that of the SW group either(9.14 vs. 9.57, P = 0.648, ηp2 = 0.002).

Regarding transfer type items, the writing groups performed significantly better than the SS group (P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.155). The SS group scored significantly lower than the SW group (4.09 vs. 7.97, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.171), and the AW group (4.09 vs. 6.97, P = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.067). However, the scores of the AW group and the SW group were not significantly different (6. 97 vs. 7.97, P = 0.077, ηp2 = 0.028) (Table 2).

Table 2 Comparison of total scores, rote-memory item scores, and transfer type item scores between groups

Data from the RAT task and the writing scores from participants in the writing groups were further analyzed. Correlation analysis for RAT scores for all three groups did show a weak positive correlation with performance in rote-memory items (r(114) = 0.18, P = 0.045). However, for participants in the writing groups, RAT scores did not show any significant correlation with the main study variables as demonstrated in Table 3.

Table 3 Correlations between main study variables

Lastly, the correlation between participants’ writing scores and their final performance was measured. Higher writing scores or performance should reflect higher levels of participation in learning. As a result, the writing scores of participants for all writing conditions combined showed a weak positive correlation with both performances in rote-memory (r(68) = 0.34, P = 0.003) and transfer type items (r(68) = 0.27, P = 0.022). However, linear regression analyses controlled for age and gender variables indicated that the writing scores significantly predicted performance on transfer type items for participants only in the AW group (R2 = 0.26, F(3, 31) = 5.123, P = 0.001) (Table 4).

Table 4 Multiple regression analysis of writing scores and performance on transfer type items

Discussion

Memorizing a large amount of knowledge still plays an important role in medical education. However, simply memorizing the given facts cannot foster students’ ability to grow as successful health professionals [27]. They need to train higher-order thinking skills to apply what they learned to novel situations as they cannot merely rely on memorization when dealing with various patients with different symptoms. This is why we investigated the effect of writing, which promotes higher-order thinking. By dividing the participants into three groups, those who study by themselves (SS), those who study by writing a summary text (SW), and those who study by writing an argumentative essay (AW), we tested each group’s performance on both rote-memory and the transfer of knowledge. While replicating previous literature on the effect of writing on students’ memory, our study has focused on finding empirical evidence that writing fosters the transfer of knowledge and higher-order thinking skills as well.

Following the first hypothesis that there would be no difference between groups in rote-memory scores, participants who learned through writing did not show significantly higher performance in rote-memory type items. These results are consistent with the previous research by Spirgel and Delaney, which reported that summary writing was not any more effective than restudying the text in solving short answer and multiple-choice questions [12]. Since the SS group learned the given material most similarly to re-studying the text, the fact that the SS group and the two writing groups performed similarly in rote-memory type items replicates previous works.

Expanding previous research on the effect of writing on learning, students were tested on their transfer of knowledge as well as how well they remember the information.  Following the second hypothesis, participants who learned through writing showed significantly higher performance in transfer type items than those who engaged in self-studying. Compared to students who just read the given material to comprehend and memorize the information, students who were involved in the writing task had to analyze what they had read and present what they had learned in their own words. Therefore, writing which requires higher-order thinking skills, such as analysis and evaluation, leads to a deeper level of learning [18] and a higher level of transfer of knowledge.

In addition to measuring the transfer of knowledge, two different writing tasks – summary and argumentative writing – were introduced in the experiment to test whether they have different effects on students’ learning. However, unlike the expectations, there were no differences between the two writing conditions, and the SW and the AW group showed similar performance in transfer type items. This pattern may be due to some characteristics of the writing task or our participant group. To elaborate, the summary writing task required participants in the SW group to delineate and summarize all four theories introduced in the given text. On the other hand, in the argumentative writing task, participants could write their claim by focusing on only one of the theories they preferred. Therefore, the SW group may have had an advantage over the AW group in answering the final test questions that dealt with the whole text. Next, the scores between the two writing groups may not have shown any difference due to the specificity of medical students. Medical students may already be familiar with writing summaries as they must have had memorized a large amount of knowledge in school. Conversely, students may lack experience in writing an argumentative essay and have weaker argumentation skills.

Results from further linear regression analysis imply similar conclusions (Table 4). Writing scores of both the SW and the AW group showed a weak positive correlation with performance in rote-memory and transfer type items (Table 3). However, linear regression results showed some different patterns on the effect of writing for the two writing groups (Fig. 1). While participants in the SW group achieved similar scores on transfer type items regardless of their writing scores, the test performance of participants in the AW group increased according to their writing scores. Since writing scores show a significant linear relationship with performance in transfer type items only in the AW condition, participants with poor writing skills in this group may not have benefited enough from the writing task. This also implies students who did not actively participate in the argumentative writing task may have lowered the average of the group performance, regardless of how argumentative writing promotes deeper learning than summary writing. Meanwhile, no significant relationship between writing and performance in transfer type items was found in the SW group, which suggests there was little individual variance in the learning effect of summary writing. Such different dynamics between the two different types of writing could have resulted in lower test scores in the AW group, or higher test scores in the SW group, bringing academic performance within the two writing groups to a similar level.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Linear Regression between writing scores and academic performance. Regression results representing how writing scores (0–10) affect students’ academic performance on transfer type items(0–16) for the two writing groups. Red points indicating summary writing(SW) group; blue points indicating argumentative writing(AW) group. For participants in SW groups, no clear pattern was found between performance and writing scores. On the other hand, participants in the AW group showed a linear relationship between transfer type item scores and writing scores

Last but not least, further analyses between RAT scores and the main study variables were performed to see whether creativity, or creative thinking which falls under higher-order thinking, is related to writing and final performance. RAT scores were only positively correlated with scores for rote-memory type items. This may be attributed to the fact that RAT focuses on measuring convergent thinking abilities, and not divergent thinking [28]. Creativity mainly consists of convergent thinking and divergent thinking; however, these two concepts are different. While convergent thinking skills involve the production of a single predetermined solution to a given problem like RAT, divergent thinking skills require the exploration of multiple possible solutions to generate creative ideas. In this sense, transfer type items that require the application of knowledge in various contexts may be closely related to divergent thinking than convergent thinking. The different focus of each assessment tool could have been the reason why the RAT did not show any significant correlations with the writing scores or performance in transfer type items.

To sum up, we were able to find some empirical evidence that writing can be a useful learning activity for medical students. First, we were able to replicate previous research by showing those who studied through writing performed similarly with those who self-studied the text in rote-memory items. Second, we went further to show writing has a significant influence on the level of transfer of knowledge for both summary and argumentative tasks, although no significant difference was found between the two conditions.

This study, however, still has some limitations. First, overall performance scores showed a floor effect. Specifically for transfer type items, the mean score of the three groups ranged from 4 to 7 points, much below the maximum of 16 points. Such an effect could have been due to the unfamiliar subject matter or high difficulty level of the items, which were selected from the National teacher certification examination in Korea. Nevertheless, the writing groups achieved significantly higher scores in transfer type items compared to the SS group. For future studies, it would be meaningful to replicate our results with a larger sample size and materials more closely related to what medical students study within the curriculum. Also, a more relevant measure of cognitive processes involved with higher-order thinking must be considered. The task used in our study was the RAT, which assesses creativity, specifically convergent thinking skills. Since almost no significant correlations were found between RAT scores and the main study variables, implementing and explaining other measurements will help us better understand the cognitive processes behind the effect of writing on learning. Lastly, due to time constraints, we could not fully investigate the effect of writing on academic performance in the long run. As we have seen from the linear regression results, students with weaker argumentation skills may not have fully benefited from the writing activity. Thus, if we help students increase their argumentation skills and practice argumentative writing over several periods, the benefits of writing on learning may turn out to be bigger. In this sense, it would be worthy of investigating whether the AW group shows better performance than the SW group after more training, and experiment with what other long-term benefits writing can bring to the medical students.

The current study provided new evidence to encourage the use of writing to complement the traditional ways of teaching in the medical education curriculum. Teachers are therefore encouraged to utilize writing in the classroom to help students develop thinking and apply what they learned to novel situations.

Conclusion

Writing could be used as a useful learning tool that promotes higher-order thinking. As students have to analyze the given information and evaluate it to express their ideas in a compact piece of writing, higher-order thinking is promoted throughout the process. Therefore, our findings provide empirical evidence for writing to be adopted in medical classroom settings for greater benefits, in light of the transfer of knowledge. Overall, students who learned through writing showed a similar level of memorization with those who engaged in self-studying but higher performance in transfer type items. However, no particular difference in performance between SW and AW group was found, suggesting future studies are needed to identify what specific writing activities are helpful for medical school students. All in all, by actively using writing assignments in class, we expect medical students to acquire knowledge and foster higher-order thinking skills at the same time.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

SS:

Self-study group

SW:

Summary writing group

AW:

Argumentative writing group

RAT:

Remote Associates Test

References

  1. Zinsser WK. Writing to learn. New York: HarperCollins Publishers; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Nickerson RS, Perkins DN, Smith EE. The teaching of thinking. Abingdon: Routledge; 2014.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Liu X. Essentials of science classroom assessment. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Quirk ME. Intuition and metacognition in medical education keys to developing expertise. New York: Springer; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Davidson JE, Deuser R, Sternberg RJ. The role of metacognition in problem solving. Metacognition: Knowing about knowing; 1994. p. 207–26.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Hong WH, Vadivelu J, Daniel EGS, Sim JH. Thinking about thinking: changes in first-year medical students’ metacognition and its relation to performance. Med Educ Online. 2015;20(1):27561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Turan S, Demirel O, Sayek I. Metacognitive awareness and self-regulated learning skills of medical students in different medical curricula. Med Teach. 2009;31(10):e477–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Kim S. Portfolio assessment and its application in medical education. Korean J Med Educ. 2003;15(2):73–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Davis M, Hult RE. Effects of writing summaries as a generative learning activity during note taking. Teach Psychol. 1997;24(1):47–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. King JR, Biggs S, Lipsky S. Students’ self-questioning and summarizing as reading study strategies. J Read Behav. 1984;16(3):205–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Penrose AM. To write or not to write: effects of task and task interpretation on learning through writing. Writ Commun. 1992;9(4):465–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Spirgel AS, Delaney PF. Does writing summaries improve memory for text? Educ Psychol Rev. 2016;28(1):171–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Goldstone RL, Day SB. Introduction to “new conceptualizations of transfer of learning”. Educ Psychol. 2012;47(3):149–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Boscolo P, Mason L. Writing to learn, writing to transfer. In: Writing as a learning tool. Berlin: Springer; 2001. p. 83–104.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  15. Halpern DF. Assessing the effectiveness of critical thinking instruction. J Gen Educ. 2001;50(4):270–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Miri B, David BC, Uri Z. Purposely teaching for the promotion of higher-order thinking skills: a case of critical thinking. Res Sci Educ. 2007;37(4):353–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Pressley M, Wood E, Woloshyn VE, Martin V, King A, Menke D. Encouraging mindful use of prior knowledge: attempting to construct explanatory answers facilitates learning. Educ Psychol. 1992;27(1):91–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Jonassen DH, Kim B. Arguing to learn and learning to argue: design justifications and guidelines. Educ Technol Res Dev. 2010;58(4):439–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Deane P, Odendahl N, Quinlan T, Fowles M, Welsh C, Bivens-Tatum J. Cognitive models of writing: writing proficiency as a complex integrated skill. ETS Res Rep Ser. 2008;2008(2):i–36.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Thomas T. Developing first year students’ critical thinking skills. Asian Soc Sci. 2011;7(4):26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Swanwick K. Musical development theories revisited. Music Educ Res. 2001;3(2):227–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Lee CS, Huggins AC, Therriault DJ. A measure of creativity or intelligence? Examining internal and external structure validity evidence of the remote associates test. Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts. 2014;8(4):446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lewis A, Smith D. Defining higher order thinking. Theory Pract. 1993;32(3):131–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Mednick SA, Mednick M. Remote associates test: examiner’s manual. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Mednick SA. The remote associates test. J Creat Behav. 1968;2(3):213–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Lumley T. Assessment criteria in a large-scale writing test: what do they really mean to the raters? Lang Test. 2002;19(3):246–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Lim J, Ko H, Yang JW, et al. Active learning through discussion: ICAP framework for education in health professions. BMC Med Educ. 2019;19(1):1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Chermahini SA, Hommel B. The (b) link between creativity and dopamine: spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition. 2010;115(3):458–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not Applicable.

Funding

Not Applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors have read and approved the manuscript. Conceptualization: KS; Methodology: KS, PJ; Formal analysis: YJ, LJ; Data curation: YJ, LJ; Investigation: KS, YJ, LJ; Writing - original draft preparation: KS; Writing - review and editing: LS, IJ, PJ.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Jungjoon Ihm or Jooyong Park.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Seoul National University School of Dentistry (approval No. S-D20190016). All participants were aware that they were taking part in this research and gave informed consent in writing in addition to confirming that they would allow us to use their collected data anonymously for publication. All the data were anonymously collected and analyzed.

Consent for publication

Not Applicable.

Competing interests

All authors have no potential conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, S., Yang, J.W., Lim, J. et al. The impact of writing on academic performance for medical students. BMC Med Educ 21, 61 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-021-02485-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-021-02485-2

Keywords