Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Linguistic analysis of pediatric residency personal statements: gender differences

Abstract

Background

All US residency programs require applicants to submit personal statements. Prior studies showed gender differences in personal statement writing, which has implications for gender bias in the application process, but previous studies have not considered the dual influence of specialty-specific values on personal statement writing by applicants of each gender.

Objective

To understand gender differences in pediatric residency personal statements.

Methods

From 2017 to 2018, we performed linguistic analysis of personal statements written by interviewees at a mid-size US pediatrics residency during two prior academic years. We assessed writing tone, communal language, and agentic language. We performed t-tests to evaluate for gender differences, p < 0.05.

Results

We analyzed personal statements from 85 male and 85 female interviewees. Average word count was 676 words. Personal statements demonstrated analytic writing style with authentic and positive emotional tone. We found no gender differences in communal language for social affiliation (p = 0.31), adjectives (p = 0.49), or orientation (p = 0.48), which deviates from typical gender norms for male language use. Males used agentic language of reward more frequently (p = 0.02).

Conclusions

Findings suggest that social language is valued in pediatrics, a predominantly female specialty, regardless of applicant gender. Use of reward language by males is consistent with previous findings. Future studies should evaluate gender differences in residency applications across specialties to advance understanding of the role gender plays in the application process.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Gender disparities have been pervasive in medicine worldwide. Women historically have been admitted less frequently to medical school, paid less than men for comparable work, and promoted less often to leadership roles [1, 2]. In recent decades, women have made progress, representing more than half of all students admitted to US medical schools [3]. However, women practicing medicine continue to be paid less and promoted less than their male colleagues [2, 4,5,6].

Much of the persistent disparity may be attributed to gender biases [1, 7]. Gender biases, which are highly ingrained and often subtle, are driven by expectations of how people should behave or speak, based on assigned gender. In general, females are expected to use communal (social, relationship-oriented) language and behave in communal ways, while men are expected to use agentic (self-oriented) language and behave in agentic ways [7,8,9,10,11,12]. Communal language includes references to others (family, friend, colleague); language lacking self-assertiveness (perhaps, maybe); and relationship-oriented adjectives (affectionate, helpful). Agentic language includes psychological drives that promote the self (reward, risk, power, achievement); assertive language (think, know, always, never); and adjectives showing self-assuredness (confident, ambitious).

Numerous studies show that when women in particular fail to adhere to communal gender stereotypes in the workplace, they suffer professional advancement penalties [13,14,15,16]. These concerns are associated with many women altering their speech and behavior to avoid self-promotion, especially in specialties in which females represent the minority of the workforce or when performing traditionally “male” tasks [15, 17,18,19]. Less is known about how males working in female-dominated specialties, such as pediatrics, fare based on the language they use.

Previous studies show that gender bias begins early in a medical career, starting with the Medical Student Performance Evaluation (MSPE) and specialty-specific letters of recommendation, required for all applications to US residency programs [20,21,22,23,24]. In these performance evaluations and letters of recommendation, evaluators are more likely to describe male students using agentic standout words (e.g. “exceptional”) and as leaders with innate ability, whereas women are described as strong communicators, team members, and hard workers [21, 23, 24]. These gender biases have been shown to persist in application processes throughout the medical career, including in letters of recommendation for faculty positions and reviewer critiques for grant funding [25, 26].

While letters of recommendation provide insight into the ways that gender bias might shape faculty evaluations of the applicant, linguistic analysis of personal statements offers a different perspective in the application process—an understanding of the language and stories that applicants use to convey that they are qualified for the field to which they are applying. Notably, letters of recommendation and personal statements both have shown lack of inter-rater reliability and lack of correlation with future clinical performance [27,28,29,30,31]. Additional concerns have been raised about the utility of personal statements, as many see these essays as impersonal and lacking in authenticity [32]. Yet, most programs continue to use the personal statement in their residency selection process as a means of allowing students to demonstrate unique characteristics of their personality and “fit” within the medical specialty they have chosen to pursue [33]. Linguistic analysis of personal statements may therefore provide an important opportunity to optimize use of the personal statement in understanding the values and perspectives of residency applicants by its ability to detect subtle differences in applicant writing.

Studies that previously evaluated personal statements for gender differences in internal medicine and general surgery programs showed that applicants of both genders described the importance of agentic professional values, including appeal of problem-solving in internal medicine and technical skills in surgery, but applicants otherwise upheld gender norms for communality and agency [34, 35]. Females applying to medicine and surgery programs wrote more often about communal themes (interpersonal relationships and team-based care), and males focused more on agency (academic rigor, clinical excellence, and self-promotion) [34, 35]. These findings suggest that in writing their personal statements to residency, applicants may be subject to dual social pressures of simultaneously upholding gender norms for social and agentic language use, while also demonstrating shared professional values for communality and agency with the members of their anticipated profession.

To date, no studies have examined linguistic gender differences in personal statements for pediatric residency. This area of study is particularly intriguing because pediatrics has been one of the few predominantly female medical specialties and as such, may present unique gender expectations for its future clinicians. Additionally, the focus in pediatrics on family-centered care may attract medical students with a more communal focus in their approach to clinical care, such that this population may focus on these values more heavily than would be expected in personal statements submitted for other medical specialties. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the linguistic characteristics of pediatric residency personal statements to explore whether gender differences exist and consider the dual roles of specialty-specific values and gender norms in shaping gender differences in applicant language use.

Methods

We performed retrospective content analysis of residency personal statements submitted during two academic years, 2014–2015 and 2015–2016, for applicants who intended to enter residency during the following academic year. This study was performed at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA, within the Pediatrics Department, during the study period of November 2017 through June 2018.

As part of the application to US medical residencies, graduating medical students submit biographical information, medical school and standardized testing transcripts, a Dean’s Letter known as the Medical School Performance Evaluation (MSPE) detailing the student’s medical school performance and extracurricular involvement, letters of recommendation, a personal statement, and an applicant photograph via the Electronic Residency Application System (ERAS). Each residency program then uses its own program-determined criteria to select which applicants to invite for interview. After interviews are conducted, residency programs rank all interviewees using program-specific scoring systems, based on the different elements of the residency application and the interview, and applicants rank the residency programs at which they interviewed. Finally, the National Residency Matching Program (NRMP), utilizes a mathematical algorithm based on applicant and residency program rankings to “match” applicants into residency positions [36].

While other residency programs may use the personal statement in determining which applicants to invite for interview, our program and many others utilize only the most objective measures of residency qualification and clinical skill from the application (medical school transcripts and standardized testing scores) to determine interviewee selection. Our program then reviews the subjective portions (personal statements, MSPE, and letters of recommendation) after the interview to help determine final applicant rank.

All applications from medical students who interviewed with the pediatrics residency program in the two application cycles were eligible for inclusion, except for current residents whose personal statements were excluded. This population was purposefully chosen for this initial study examining linguistic characteristics of pediatric personal statements to help understand gender differences in the writing of the candidates most likely to be interviewed by pediatric residency programs. Current residents were excluded from this study. Exclusion of current residents was based on collaborative decision-making between the study team, pediatrics residency leadership, and the IRB in effort to reduce the risk of identification of any personal statement author by study team members who work with the residents based on written content of their personal statement, even in the absence of unique identifiers. Additionally, the only demographic information provided to the study team was applicant self-reported gender. Options for applicant gender in ERAS included, “Male,” “Female,” “Decline to answer,” and “No answer.” No applicants in the study pool selected, “Decline to answer” or “No answer” for gender, so only male and female comparisons were performed.

We used the text analysis software, Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC), previously used in numerous studies evaluating gender biases in medicine and academia [22, 24, 37, 38]. LIWC is a linguistic analysis tool that aids in the study of cognitive, emotional, and structural aspects of written and verbal speech [39, 40]. The LIWC program searches bodies of text for words and word parts that match its internal dictionary of 6400 words and word stems. Users may also manually add words to the LIWC program dictionary.

LIWC primarily reports data as percentage of word occurrence in text. For example, when examining references to “reward” in text, LIWC output is the percentage of “reward” words (including, “fulfill,” “promote”, and “benefit”) used within the text. For a subset of variables, output is reported instead as a LIWC-validated scaled score, based on standardized scores derived from large comparison samples of linguistic characteristics found in speech and writing across a variety of settings in the general US population [39, 41,42,43,44].

Measures

Linguistic dimensions, writing tone

Linguistic dimensions included word count, average words per sentence, and complex word count (defined by words longer than 6 letters). Overall writing tone of personal statements was determined using four variables based on population samples of text across a variety of settings [39, 41,42,43,44]. Writing tone variables included: [1] analytic tone (tendency to use formal or informal word choices) [2]; emotional tone (tendency to use of positive or negative emotion words) [3]; clout (tendency to use language expressing expertise or tentativeness); and [4] authenticity (tendency to use language expressing vulnerability or guardedness), as scored by the LIWC program.

Agentic & communal language

Given previous findings of gender differences in agentic and communal language for males and females [7, 9, 34, 35], agentic and communal word use were our primary variables of interest. To evaluate agentic and communal language, we selected LIWC dictionary categories used in prior studies evaluating gender differences in residency applications and other social and professional contexts [9, 10, 22, 24, 37, 38]. We also manually added dictionaries for agentic and communal language, described previously in content analysis by Madera, et al. and utilized by Li, et al. in evaluation of letters of evaluation for emergency medicine [24, 37]. Agentic words included “fulfill,” “benefit,” “success,” “achieve,” “think,” “know,” and “confident.” Communal words included “family,” “friend,” “perhaps,” “maybe,” “kind,” and “helpful.” Table 1 includes the list of example dictionary words for each category.

Table 1 Dependent Variables Examined Using Linguistic Inquiry Word Count (LIWC) [39]

Procedure

All 85 male-authored personal statements that met eligibility criteria for our study were included. In order to avoid over-sampling females (which represented over 75% of the interviewee pool), female-authored personal statements were randomly selected using random number generator to obtain an equal number of personal statements, resulting in 170 personal statements total (85 from males, 85 from females) that were then provided to the study team for analysis in de-identified form.

In the first phase of content analysis, we assessed proper LIWC word categorization for pediatric residency personal statements. Using a sample of 10 personal statements, two authors (JB, AG) independently evaluated LIWC output variables to find words that might commonly occur in personal statements but be categorized inappropriately by LIWC. For instance, “Children’s” (e.g. “Children’s Hospital”) was frequently used as a proper noun and if not excluded in this context, would have been counted inappropriately as a word of social affiliation. Similarly, the words “practice,” “admitted,” and “down” (as in Down Syndrome) have different meaning in medicine as compared to their categorization in LIWC. An investigator (AG) reviewed use of these words in all 170 personal statements in order to ensure appropriate word categorization based on context.

Analysis

Next, we entered all personal statement texts into the LIWC program and analyzed them using the LIWC dictionary variables for linguistic dimensions; writing tone; agentic language; and communal language. We then analyzed the personal statements using the manually entered dictionaries based on the work of Madera, et al. for agentic and communal language [37]. For each LIWC output variable, two-tailed t-tests (p < 0.05) were performed using STATA (STATA Software 15.0) [45] to compare mean percentage of word use and mean LIWC-validated scaled score for male- and female-authored personal statements.

IRB

The Education and Social/Behavioral Science Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin-Madison deemed this study non-human subjects research (2017–1422) and therefore exempt from its review, given that the personal statements for this study were provided to the study team for analysis without identifiers, and the identities of the writers could not be ascertained.

Results

A total of 423 applicants interviewed with our pediatrics residency program in 2015 and 2016, of whom 96 (22.7%) identified as male and 327 (77.3%) identified as female. After exclusion of current residents (11 males, 19 females), 393 personal statements were eligible for inclusion and 170 personal statements were ultimately included (43.3% of eligible personal statements).

Linguistic dimensions, writing tone

Average total word count was 676.4 words (SD = 113.31) with an average of 22.5 words per sentence (SD = 3.83). Complex language accounted for 27.82% of all analyzed personal statement text (SD = 3.68). There were no gender differences in word count (male authors 674.5, SD = 110.51, female authors 678.4, SD = 116.67, p = 0.83); words per sentence (male authors 22.25%, SD = 3.72, female authors 22.80%, SD = 3.93, p = 0.35); or complex word count (male authors 27.99%, SD = 3.59, female authors 27.64%, SD = 3.79, p = 0.54) for males compared to females.

Personal statements scored as highly analytic (LIWC scaled score 81.41, SD = 9.84, range 0–100, higher scores indicating predominantly formal writing) [39] and as demonstrating positive emotional tone (LIWC scaled score 86.44, SD = 15.56, range 0–100, higher scores indicating more frequent use of positive over negative emotion words) [39]. Scores were mid-range in measures of authenticity (LIWC scaled score 58.77 SD = 18.66, range 0–100, higher scores indicating more expressive writing) [39]. Scores were mid-range for clout, suggesting equal balance in using authoritative and tentative language (LIWC scaled score of 51.11 SD = 13.59, range 0–100, higher scores indicating more frequent use of language expressing expertise) [39]. There were no significant differences in LIWC scaled scores for males and females for any of the writing tone variables, p > 0.05. See Appendix A for detailed results.

Agentic language

Males used agentic language expressing the psychological drive of reward more frequently than females, accounting for 1.80% (SD = 0.73) of all words written by males and 1.55% (SD = 0.67) of words written by females, (p = 0.02). The range of reward word frequency used by applicants was consistent with the percentage of reward words typically used by the general population in natural speech (1.73%) and expressive writing (1.56%) [39]. Commonly identified words of reward in this study included, “fulfillment,” “rewarding,” and “opportunity.” Table 2 demonstrates excerpts of reward language.

Table 2 Example Excerpts of Reward Language in Pediatric Residency Personal Statements

There were no differences in other language of agency between males and females, including words for risk (male authors 0.45%, SD = 0.82, female authors 0.44%, SD = 0.31, p = 0.91), power (male authors 3.37%, SD = 0.95, female authors 3.45%, SD = 0.95 p = 0.58), risk (p = 0.82), achievement (male authors 3.27%, SD = 1.01, female authors 3.13%, SD = 1.07 p = 0.38), insight (male authors 3.19%, SD = 1.01, female authors 3.26%, SD = 1.04, p = 0.66), and certainty (male authors 1.24%, SD = 0.59, female authors 1.16%, SD = 0.51, p = 0.34). Evaluation of agentic language using the manually entered dictionary for agentic words likewise showed no gender differences in the use of agentic adjectives (assertive, confident, ambitious), (male authors 0.05%, SD = 0.10, female authors 0.03%, SD = 0.07, p = 0.30) or orientation (earn, gain, do), (male authors 0.28%, SD = 0.29, female authors 0.23%, SD = 0.23, p = 0.21). See Appendix B for detailed results.

Communal language

There was no statistical difference between male and female authors in the use of any variables measuring communal language. Language of social affiliation accounted for 2.79% words written by males (SD = 1.07), and 2.95% of words written by females (SD = 1.02), (p = 0.31), slightly higher than that which is typically found in natural speech (2.06%), expressive writing (2.45%), and blogs (2.20%) for the general population [39]. References to family accounted for 0.99% of text written by males (SD = 0.65) and 1.14% written by females (SD = 0.79), (p = 0.20). References to friends accounted for < 0.5% words written by both males and females, (male authors 0.14%, SD = 0.17; female authors 0.11, SD = 0.14; p = 0.17). There were no differences in male references (male authors 0.98%, SD = 1.08; female authors 0.98%, SD = 0.99; p = 0.99) and female references (male authors 0.81%, SD = 0.91; female authors 0.88%, SD = 0.54; p = 0.59).

Tentative language was used with similar occurrence by males and females, accounting for 1.48%, of words written by males (SD = 0.68) and 1.31% of words written by females (SD = 0.57), (p = 0.09). Evaluation of communal language using the manually entered dictionary for communal words likewise showed no statistical gender differences in the use of communal adjectives (affectionate, nurturing, helpful, kind), (male authors 0.11%, SD = 0.16; female authors 0.12%, SD = 0.13, p = 0.49) or orientation (husband, wife, baby, colleague), (male authors 1.54%, SD = 0.75; female authors 1.62%, SD = 0.75, p = 0.48). Table 3 demonstrates excerpts of communal language. See Appendix B for detailed results.

Table 3 Example Excerpts of Communal Language in Pediatric Residency Personal Statements

Discussion

Our study found that applicants to pediatrics residency wrote their personal statements with a highly analytical and moderately authentic style with positive emotional tone. Both males and females wrote in a non-assertive way, striking a tone that evenly balanced expertise and tentativeness.

The most notable finding in our study was that males applying to pediatrics used communal language on par with their female counterparts. This finding deviates from typical gender norm expectations, which would predict that males use social language less than females [7,8,9,10,11,12] and suggests that personal statement language may be partially dictated by the applicant’s perception of specialty-specific values, regardless of gender norms for language use.

Students rotating through pediatrics, for example, are taught that family-centeredness is the gold standard in providing pediatric care [46,47,48]. Students receive feedback on their family-centered rounding skills and may even be required to attend workshops on optimizing family-centered care [46, 47]. This emphasis and evaluation of the pediatric trainee’s skills in relationship-centeredness demonstrates to the student applicant that communal traits are not only expected but highly valued by faculty in pediatrics. Additionally, mentor physicians helping to review and edit personal statements may encourage this career assimilation as they assist applicants in revising their personal statements.

Moreover, when males represent the minority in a workplace (36.6% male workforce in pediatrics in the US), social and professional pressure may exist that encourages communal behavior and speech (traits more often associated with females in prior studies) [7,8,9,10,11]. Studies by Cejka and Eagly have shown that “to the extent that occupations were female dominated, feminine personality or physical attributes were thought more essential for success; to the extent that occupations were male dominated, masculine personality or physical attributes were thought more essential” [49]. Communal attributes may be highly valued by the male applicants who choose to pursue a career in pediatrics; however males entering pediatrics might also be subject to subtle social and professional pressure to include anecdotes of communal relationships and utilize communal language more frequently than typical gender norms would predict.

The finding that applicants veer from typical linguistic gender norms as a means of describing alignment with specialty-specific values is consistent with the findings of Osman et al. and Ostapenko, et al. which previously examined personal statements for internal medicine and general surgery residencies [34, 35]. While the primary aims of these studies were not to examine the influence of specialty-specific values on personal statement writing, the studies did find that the primary themes described by both male and female applicants were themes of agency, which gender norms would not typically predict for females. Unlike pediatrics, internal medicine and general surgery are male-dominated fields (62.1 and 79.4% male workforces in the US, respectively) [50]. As such, the cultural values of these specialties likely favors an image of agency overall (i.e. academic rigor, technical expertise) [7], implicitly encouraging applicants, regardless of gender, to conform to these expectations.

In our study, we also found that applicants did not completely deviate from gender norms for language use. Males in our study used agentic language of reward more than females, consistent with gender expectations. This finding is also consistent with findings from internal medicine and surgery personal statements in which gender differences in minor themes were found (females described the importance of teamwork and communication, males utilized self-promoting anecdotes and descriptions of technical expertise) [34, 35]. These findings suggest that despite the tendency to appeal to specialty-specific values for the majority of personal statement content, the use of gender normative language (agency for males, communality for females) may persist in subtle ways, demonstrating the potential pervasiveness of gender expectations on the applicant.

There are a few limitations to this study. The first is that although the LIWC program has been validated in social psychology literature and shown to have good reliability in using “marker words” in place of content coding [10], we could have missed words or ideas that would have been otherwise identified if context was considered. Additionally, our study population was drawn from a single US medical school and from only those who interviewed with our program, rather than all applicants. Although our program’s approach to interviewee selection, which utilizes only the most objective parts of the application (scores, transcripts), significantly reduces the risk that our sample selected for a population that demonstrated program-preferred values (eg. communality), this remains possible. Additionally, by focusing on this population, we were able to describe the linguistic characteristics of applicants who would most likely be interviewed at a pediatric US residency program based on objective measures of achievement in US and international medical schools (scores, transcripts). However, we were unable to determine whether certain linguistic characteristics in personal statements are preferred and selected for by reviewers in the interview selection process, which may vary internationally and across cultures. Finally, our study cannot be generalized to other fields of medicine; however, as there remains a current gap in the literature focusing on understanding how language patterns vary by specialty, generalizations across specialties should not be made until additional studies are undertaken specifically examining each specialty.

This is the first study to our knowledge that has characterized the linguistic qualities and gender differences of pediatric residency personal statements and considered ways in which professional specialty choice and gender expectations might dually shape the way an applicant writes. It is important for residency training programs to understand the potential implications of these subtle language findings in personal statement writing for two reasons. First, understanding the social pressures that may influence an applicant’s writing may help training programs determine how and whether to utilize the personal statement in their applicant ranking process, knowing that in addition to its known limitations as a tool to predict future resident success, this subjective element of the application may result in unintended bias in the evaluation of the applicant’s “fit” for the program [28,29,30,31]. If residency programs are not aware of the potential for bias in evaluating the personal statement, they may inadvertently exclude highly qualified applicants based on subtle qualities of applicants’ writing that do not reflect their skills as physicians. Additionally, understanding the role of societal gender norms and expectations for communal language use by females and agentic language use by males and how those expectations may shift within a medical specialty will allow educators to consider ways in which these expectations enter the application process, particularly when reviewers are not attuned to their own expectations and bias, as well as other educational settings.

The findings of this study also has potential practical implications for residency programs. Residency programs that utilize the personal statement in their selection process might consider methods of limiting gender bias in evaluation of the personal statement by blinding reviewers to applicant biographical information and photograph or creating other standardized measures of personal statement evaluation and scoring [31]. Additionally, this study sheds light on the potential consideration of whether linguistic analysis could be utilized in the evaluation of personal statements to help programs identify candidates that possess positive interpersonal and professional traits that align with program values. This approach could represent a standardized approach in evaluating personal statements that when applied to all personal statements, indiscriminate of applicant gender, could select for the more intangible applicant interpersonal characteristics that programs may seek (eg. communality in pediatrics). However, programs would have to carefully consider the ways in which utilizing this method could also unintentionally perpetuate systematic or cultural bias, and future studies would be required to investigate validity of this approach in screening applicants.

In order to further understand the way specialty-specific expectations and gender norms enter the residency application process across medical fields and applicant populations, it would be important to perform linguistic analysis of personal statements from a wide array of specialties, including male-dominated fields (orthopedics, urology) and female-dominated fields (obstetrics), as well as differences in highly ranked applicants as compared to lower or unranked applicants to determine if certain linguistic characteristics correlate with applicant ranking. Additionally, future studies could focus on applicants who identify as gender non-conforming (nonbinary, transgender identity), in order to understand the true breadth of gender differences in the application process. Finally, other applicant characteristics, such as ethnicity and country of origin could be included in future analyses. Doing so could provide residency programs with more complete understanding of how gender biases operate across cultures and empower residency programs to optimize diversity, inclusion, and equity in the application process.

Conclusions

In writing a personal statement for application to pediatric residency programs, applicants may be subject to dual pressures of upholding gender norms while also demonstrating shared professional values with the members of their anticipated profession. While applicants may utilize language that deviates from gender expectations for communality and agency in order to convey their “fit” in their anticipated career, gender normative language likely persists in personal statements in subtle ways. Evaluation of linguistic gender differences in highly ranked and low-ranking applicants, across medical specialties, and applicants with different cultural backgrounds would help expand understanding of how specialty-specific values and gender expectations dually influence the residency selection process and allow for possible gender bias that may persist throughout the physician’s career. With improved understanding of these subtle social processes, we will be better able to provide education about implicit bias and consider ways to reduce bias in the residency selection process, with the ultimate goal of cultivating medical training systems that reduce the negative impact of these biases.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to them containing information that could compromise privacy protections, and restrictions apply to the availability of the data, but data may be available from the corresponding author on reasonable request and if additional permissions are granted.

References

  1. Wright AL, Schwindt LA, Bassford TL, Reyna VF, Shisslak CM, St Germain PA, et al. Gender differences in academic advancement: patterns, causes, and potential solutions in one US College of medicine. Acad Med. 2003;78(5):500–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Rotbart HA, McMillen D, Taussig H, Daniels SR. Assessing gender equity in a large academic department of pediatrics. Acad Med. 2012;87(1):98–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. U.S. Medical School Applicants and Matriculants by School, State of Legal Residence, and Sex, 2018-2019: Association of American Medical Colleges; [Available from: https://www.aamc.org/data/facts/applicantmatriculant.

  4. Weaver AC, Wetterneck TB, Whelan CT, Hinami K. A matter of priorities? Exploring the persistent gender pay gap in hospital medicine. J Hosp Med. 2015;10(8):486–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Desai T, Ali S, Fang X, Thompson W, Jawa P, Vachharajani T. Equal work for unequal pay: the gender reimbursement gap for healthcare providers in the United States. Postgrad Med J. 2016;92(1092):571–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Jena AB, Khullar D, Ho O, Olenski AR, Blumenthal DM. Sex differences in academic rank in US medical schools in 2014. Jama-J Am Med Assoc. 2015;314(11):1149–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Carnes M, Bartels CM, Isaac C, Kaatz A, Kolehmainen C. Why is John more likely to become department chair than Jennifer? Trans Am Clin Climatol Assoc. 2015;126:197–214.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Eagly AH, Johannesen-Schmidt MC. The leadership styles of women and men. J Soc Issues. 2001;57(4):781–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Iosub D, Laniado D, Castillo C, Fuster Morell M, Kaltenbrunner A. Emotions under discussion: gender, status and communication in online collaboration. PLoS One. 2014;9(8):e104880.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Schultheiss OC. Are implicit motives revealed in mere words? Testing the marker-word hypothesis with computer-based text analysis Frontiers in psychology, vol. 4; 2013. p. 748.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Bernzweig J, Takayama JI, Phibbs C, Lewis C, Pantell RH. Gender differences in physician-patient communication. Evidence from pediatric visits. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1997;151(6):586–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Menegatti M, Rubini M. Gender bias and sexism in language. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Communication. 2017. Retrieved 12 Sept. 2018, from https://oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228613-e-470.

  13. Eagly AH, Karau SJ. Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychol Rev. 2002;109(3):573–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Heilman ME. Description and prescription: how gender stereotypes prevent Women’s ascent up the organizational ladder. J Soc Issues. 2001;57(4):657–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Heilman ME, Okimoto TG. Why are women penalized for success at male tasks?: the implied communality deficit. J Appl Psychol. 2007;92(1):81–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Moss-Racusin CA, Rudman LA. Disruptions in Women’s self-promotion: the backlash avoidance model. Psychol Women Q. 2010;34(2):186–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Kolehmainen C, Brennan M, Filut A, Isaac C, Carnes M. Afraid of being “witchy with a ‘b’”: a qualitative study of how gender influences residents’ experiences leading cardiopulmonary resuscitation. Acad Med. 2014;89(9):1276–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Girod S, Fassiotto M, Grewal D, Ku MC, Sriram N, Nosek BA, et al. Reducing implicit gender leadership Bias in academic medicine with an educational intervention. Acad Med. 2016;91(8):1143–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Fassiotto M, Li J, Maldonado Y, Kothary N. Female surgeons as counter stereotype: the impact of gender perceptions on trainee evaluations of physician faculty. J Surg Educ. 2018;75(5):1140–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Ross DA, Boatright D, Nunez-Smith M, Jordan A, Chekroud A, Moore EZ. Differences in words used to describe racial and gender groups in medical student performance evaluations. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0181659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Turrentine FE, Dreisbach CN, St Ivany AR, Hanks JB, Schroen AT. Influence of Gender on Surgical Residency Applicants’ Recommenedation Letters. J Am Coll Surg. 2019;228(4):356–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Isaac C, Chertoff J, Lee B, Carnes M. Do students’ and authors’ genders affect evaluations? A linguistic analysis of medical student performance evaluations. Acad Med. 2011;86(1):59–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Friedman R, Fang CH, Hasbun J, Han H, Mady LJ, Eloy JA, et al. Use of standardized letters of recommendation for otolaryngology head and neck surgery residency and the impact of gender. Laryngoscope. 2017;127(12):2738–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Li S, Fant AL, McCarthy DM, Miller D, Craig J, Kontrick A. Gender differences in language of standardized letter of evaluation narratives for emergency medicine residency applicants. AEM Educ Train. 2017;1(4):334–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Trix F, Psenka C. Exploring the color of glass: letters of recommendation for female and male medical faculty. Discourse Soc. 2016;14(2):191–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Kaatz A, Magua W, Zimmerman DR, Carnes M. A quantitative linguistic analysis of National Institutes of Health R01 application critiques from investigators at one institution. Acad Med. 2015;90(1):69–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Bajwa NM, Yudkowsky R, Belli D, Vu NV, Park YS. Validity evidence for a residency admissions standardized assessment letter for pediatrics. Teach Learn Med. 2018;30(2):173–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. White BA, Sadoski M, Thomas S, Shabahang M. Is the evaluation of the personal statement a reliable component of the general surgery residency application? J Surg Educ. 2012;69(3):340–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kulatunga-Moruzi C, Norman GR. Validity of admissions measures in predicting performance outcomes: the contribution of cognitive and non-cognitive dimensions. Teach Learn Med. 2002;14(1):34–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Badal JJ, Jacobsen WK, Holt BW. Behavioral evaluations of anesthesiology residents and overuse of the first-person pronoun in personal statements. J Grad Med Educ. 2011;3(2):151–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Lee AG, Golnik KC, Oetting TA, Beaver HA, Boldt HC, Olson R, et al. Re-engineering the resident applicant selection process in ophthalmology: a literature review and recommendations for improvement. Surv Ophthalmol. 2008;53(2):164–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Max BA, Gelfand B, Brooks MR, Beckerly R, Segal S. Have personal statements become impersonal? An evaluation of personal statements in anesthesiology residency applications. J Clin Anesth. 2010;22(5):346–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Smith EA, Weyhing B, Mody Y, Smith WL. A critical analysis of personal statements submitted by radiology residency applicants. Acad Radiol. 2005;12(8):1024–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Osman NY, Schonhardt-Bailey C, Walling JL, Katz JT, Alexander EK. Textual analysis of internal medicine residency personal statements: themes and gender differences. Med Educ. 2015;49(1):93–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Ostapenko L, Schonhardt-Bailey C, Sublette JW, Smink DS, Osman NY. Textual analysis of general surgery residency personal statements: topics and gender differences. J Surg Educ. 2018;75(3):573–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. 2002–2019 National Resident Matching Program: How the Matching Algorithm Works [Internet]. NRMP International. [cited 8/15/19]. Available from: http://www.nrmp.org/matching-algorithm/.

  37. Madera JM, Hebl MR, Martin RC. Gender and letters of recommendation for academia: agentic and communal differences. J Appl Psychol. 2009;94(6):1591–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Schmader T, Whitehead J, Wysocki VH. A linguistic comparison of letters of recommendation for male and female chemistry and biochemistry job applicants. Sex Roles. 2007;57(7–8):509–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Pennebaker JW, Boyd RL, Jordan K, Blackburn K. The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  40. MR M. Quantitative text analysis. In: Eid M, Diener E, editors. Handbook of multimethod measurement in psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Kacewicz E, Pennebaker JW, Davis M, Jeon M, Graesser AC. Pronoun use reflects standings in social hierarchies. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2014;33(2):125–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Pennebaker JW, Chung CK, Frazee J, Lavergne GM, Beaver DI. When Small Words Foretell Academic Success: The Case of College Admissions Essays. PLoS One. 2014;9(12):e115844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Newman ML, Pennebaker JW, Berry DS, Richards JM. Lying words: predicting deception from linguistic styles. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29(5):665–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Cohn MA, Mehl MR, Pennebaker JW. Linguistic markers of psychological change surrounding September 11, 2001. Psychol Sci. 2004;15(10):687–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. STATA Software [Available from: https://www.stata.com/.

  46. Rao P, Hill E, Palka C, Rea K, Jones K, Balzer K, et al. Improving pediatric resident communication during family-centered rounds using a novel simulation-based curriculum. MedEdPORTAL. 2018;14:10733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Zak S, Alcamo A, Herrmann L, et al. Medical student presentations on family-centered rounds: a workshop to teach an art form. MedEdPORTAL. 2016;12:10394.

  48. IFPF-CC COHC. Patient- and family-centered care and the pediatrician's role. Pediatrics. 2012;129(2):394–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Cejka MA, Eagly AH. Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2016;25(4):413–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. AAMC. Data and Reports - Workforce - Data and Analysis - AAMC Figure 1.3 Percentage of active Physicians Who Are Female by Specialty, 2017 [Available from: https://www.aamc.org/data/workforce/reports/492560/1-3chart.html.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Christine Richards for assistance with formatting and citations.

Funding

No funding was provided for the collection, analysis, or interpretation of data for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JB substantially contributed to the conception and design of the work; the analysis and interpretation of the personal statement data; and the drafting and revising of the content in this manuscript. AG substantially contributed to the conception and design of the work; the analysis and interpretation of the personal statement data; and the drafting and revising of the content in this manuscript. JF substantially contributed to the analysis and interpretation of the personal statement data; and the drafting and revising of the content in this manuscript. MAM substantially contributed to the conception and design of the work; the analysis and interpretation of the personal statement data; and the drafting and revising of the content in this manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica C. Babal.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Education and Social/Behavioral Science Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin-Madison deemed this study non-human subjects research (2017–1422) and therefore exempt from its review, given that the personal statements for this study were provided to the study team for analysis without identifiers, and the identities of the writers could not be ascertained.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix A

Table 4 LIWC Scaled Scores for Pediatric Personal Statement Writing Tone Variables

Appendix B

Table 5 Percentage of Pediatric Personal Statement Text Containing Agentic and Communal Language

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Babal, J.C., Gower, A.D., Frohna, J.G. et al. Linguistic analysis of pediatric residency personal statements: gender differences. BMC Med Educ 19, 392 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-019-1838-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-019-1838-x

Keywords