Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Do poor patients suffer from inaccurate diagnoses more than well-to-do patients? A randomized control trial

Abstract

Background

Poor patients have greater morbidity and die up to 10 years earlier than patients who have higher socio-economic status. These findings are often attributed to differences in life-style between groups. The present study aimed at investigating the extent to which physicians contribute to the effect by providing relative poorer care, resulting in relative neglect in terms of time spent with a poor patient and more inaccurate diagnoses.

Methods

A randomised experiment with 45 internal medicine residents. Doctors diagnosed 12 written clinical vignettes that were exactly the same except for the description of the patients’ socio-economic status. Each participant diagnosed four of the vignettes in a poor-patient version, four in a rich-patient version, and four in a version that did not contain socio-economic markers, in a balanced within-subjects incomplete block design. Main measurements were: diagnostic accuracy scores and time spent on diagnosis.

Results

Mean diagnostic accuracy scores (range 0–1) did not significantly differ among the conditions of the experiment (for poor patients: 0.48; for rich patients: 0.52; for patients without socio-economic markers: 0.54; p > 0.05). While confronted with patients not presenting with socio-economic background information, the participants spent significantly less time-to-diagnosis ((for poor patients: 168 s; for rich patients: 176 s; for patients without socio-economic markers: 151 s; p < 0.01), however due to the fact that the former vignettes were shorter.

Conclusion

There is no reason to believe that physicians are prejudiced against poor patients and therefore treat them differently from rich patients or patients without discernible socio-economic background.

Peer Review reports

Background

Poorly educated people are generally in poorer health than the better educated [1,2,3,4,5]. They engage in unhealthy behaviors, such as smoking, alcohol consumption, and physical inactivity [3], are suffering from stress to a larger extent [6,7,8], and consume unhealthy food more often [9]. In addition, people in the most-disadvantaged socio-economic-status quartile have an almost three-fold increased risk of mortality relative to those in the least-disadvantaged quartile [3]. On average, poor people die up to 10 years earlier than their more well-to-do counterparts [2].

Studies in search of causal factors have concentrated on the lifestyle factors mentioned above. Less is known yet about institutional factors that contribute to poor health, such as differential access to health care facilities [10], or late-stage diagnosis of disease [11]. Even less is known about how physicians deal with poor, as opposed to well-to-do, patients. It is possible that part of the greater morbidity and mortality of poor people is due to relative neglect of their doctors.

There are some studies that seem to imply that this is indeed the case, for instance in referral to psychotherapy [12], or in the diagnosis of late-stage breast cancer [13]. Rathore, et al. [14] found that poor patients were treated less adequately when acute myocardial infarction was concerned. These studies were however observational in nature. We only came across two studies in which social class was experimentally controlled. In the first study [15], participating primary care doctors viewed two video-vignettes of a scripted consultation in which the patient presented with standardized symptoms of coronary heart disease. Videotapes were identical apart from varying patients’ gender, age, class and race. Gender of patient significantly influenced doctors’ diagnostic and management activities. However, there was no influence of social class, neither on the doctor’s diagnosis, nor on the management activities undertaken. It is however possible that the actors playing the various roles were not able to present sufficiently different personalities. In a second study [16], our own research group presented residents in internal medicine with identical case vignettes for diagnosis. However, half of the subjects received the vignettes accompanied by a picture of a clearly poor person—clothes looked second-hand and contained rips; faces were dirty—while the other half received a picture of the same person, but in clearly well-to-do clothing. The expectation was that poor-looking patients would receive less attention (as measured by the amount of time needed to reach a diagnosis) and would be subject to less accurate diagnosis. However, we failed to find such differences.

In hindsight, the latter study had three potential shortcomings. The first was that we only presented two patients—a man and a woman in two different guises-- rather than a more diverse group of patients. The second was that a more or less classless version of our patients was not included in the design. It is possible that social class information—whatever its nature--influences diagnostic decision making mainly by increasing the number of possible alternative diagnoses. For instance, a poor miner plagued with headaches may lead the doctor to inappropriately think of lung-related diseases, whereas the presentation of an obese businessman with headaches may lead him or her to think of cardiovascular causes. A presentation of the same patient without class-related characteristics may not be burdened by such potentially unproductive hypotheses. And the third shortcoming was similar to the Arber, et al. [15] study, in that the patients still may have looked too much alike. It is for instance possible that doctors deduce social class from characteristics other than poor clothing or dirt on faces. We therefore, decided to follow a strategy that we successfully employed in two earlier studies on the effect of difficult patients on diagnostic decision-making [17, 18]: we did not show videos or pictures of the patients, but described them.

This article reports results of an experiment in which physicians were confronted with 12 vignettes of patients of different socio-economic status but with the same symptoms and the same underlying diseases. Using a within-subjects design, each participant was confronted, in random order, with four poor, four rich, and four patients for whom no class information was available. The number of accurate diagnoses, and time needed to reach a diagnosis, were recorded.

Method

Participants

Participants were 45 internal medicine residents (mean age = 28.91; standard deviation = 2.45; 31 male) from the College of Medicine, King Saud bin Abdelaziz University for Health Sciences, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia. The residents had on average 3.60 years of post-medical-school experience (standard deviation = 1.90). All 60 residents of this group were invited to participate in the study between December 2012 and March 2014, and volunteers were recruited. No incentive was provided for participation. The ethics review committee from King Abdullah International Medical Research Center (KAIMRC), approved this study. As the nature of the experiment prevented disclosure of its objectives beforehand, participants were informed about their tasks and debriefed later. All participants signed consent to use their data.

Materials

Twelve clinical cases, prepared by one of the senior researchers (M.E.M.) and based on cases used in previous studies [17,18,19], were employed in this experiment. All cases had a confirmed diagnosis and consisted of a brief description of a patient’s history, complaints, symptoms, and findings from physical examination and tests. Table 1 contains the 12 diagnoses.

Table 1 Diseases involved in the study; descriptions of rich and poor patients

In each case, a few sentences described aspects of the patient’s socio-economic status. These sentences portrayed either a patient of apparently high socio-economic position, a patient of apparently low socio-economic position, or a patient without any socio-economic markers (called “rich,” “poor,” and “neutral” patients from here), effectively producing three versions of the same clinical case. Two co-authors (I.A.; M.E.M.) prepared the descriptions based on the kind of patients one would see in the consulting room in Saudi Arabia. Table 1 also contains the short descriptions of rich and poor patients included. The neutral patient description only contained age information. In all other respects the different versions were identical, leading to the same diagnosis. Table 2 presents an example of three versions of the same case.

Table 2 Example of three versions of a clinical case

Procedure

The study employed a within-subjects design. A full within-subjects design would imply the presentation of all three versions of each case to the participants. Such presentation of three versions of the same case would however likely lead to carry-over effects: when one has seen one version, diagnosing the second, or a third, may become easier. An alternative is to present to each participant one-third of the cases in rich, one-third in poor, and one-third of the cases in neutral format, however in different combinations. In other words: Every participant received four rich patients, four poor patients, and four neutral patients such that all 12 diseases were seen once. For instance, if A1 represents a rich-patient version of diagnosis A, A2 its poor version and A3 its neutral version, then the first participant would receive the cases A1, B2, C3, D1, E2, and F3, etc., whereas the second participant would receive the cases A2, B3, C1, D2, E3, and F1, etc. Such balanced within-subjects incomplete block design enabled us to compare mean diagnostic performance scores and time-to diagnosis under the three experimental conditions.

The cases were presented on a computer screen using Qualtrics software (Qualtrics XM Platform™) First, they were informed that the study aimed to better understand the nature of clinical problem-solving in Internal Medicine. Second, they were informed that their responses were anonymous since no identifying information would be collected and that their results would have no implications for their work. Their task was to diagnose the clinical cases presented shortly. All cases were based on real patients and had a confirmed diagnosis.

Further, they were asked to work as quickly as possible; suggesting that a first impression is often correct. They should, however, not compromise accuracy. They were instructed to type only one complete and precise diagnosis which they found to be the MOST accurate for the case presented. They were also informed that once they clicked to the next case, they could not go back to previous screens. After being informed, they received a practice case, unrelated to the hypotheses tested.

Data analysis

The accuracy of participants’ diagnoses was evaluated by considering the confirmed diagnosis of each case as a standard. Two physicians (I.A.; M.E.M.) independently evaluated each diagnosis, without knowing the condition under which it was provided, as correct, partially correct, or incorrect (scored as 1, 0.5, or 0 points, respectively). A response was considered correct whenever it mentioned the core diagnosis, and partially correct when the core diagnosis was not cited but a constituent element of the diagnosis was mentioned. For example, in a case of gastric cancer, “Gastric malignancy” was considered correct, and “Malignancy; most likely colorectal cancer” was evaluated as partially correct. The two experts agreed in 85% of the diagnoses and solved discrepancies through discussion.

A repeated-measures ANOVA with patient socio-economic status (rich vs poor vs neutral) as within-subjects factors was performed on the mean diagnostic accuracy scores. This analysis tested the hypothesis that the description of poor patients would negatively affect diagnostic accuracy. To check whether the description of poor patients led doctors to speed up the diagnostic process, we performed a repeated-measures ANOVA with socio-economic status as a within-subjects factor on time spent to make the diagnosis. Significance levels were set at p < 0.05 for all comparisons. SPSS version 24.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois) was used for the statistical analyses.

Results

Table 3 contains the findings from this study.

Table 3 Mean diagnostic accuracy scores (range 0–1; standard deviations into brackets) and mean time spent in diagnosing the written clinical cases (seconds) as a function of patients’ socio-economic status, N = 45

Participants made more mistakes when diagnosing poor patient cases relative to neutral or rich patient cases, but this effect did not reach statistical significance: F (2, 43) = 0.47, p > .05. Time spent on diagnosis did however significantly differ, F (2, 43) = 7.57, p < .01. Participants needed significantly less time to diagnose neutral patients; this effect was however entirely due to the fact that the neutral patient versions were shorter than the other cases: F (2, 43) = 2.27, p > .05.

Discussion

Poor patients die much earlier than their more well-to-do fellow citizens [2, 3]. The question central to the present study was the extent to which doctor neglect contributes to this empirical finding. To that end, 45 residents of a Saudi medical school were presented with 12 cases in three different versions. One version described a rich patient, 1 a poor patient, and the third a patient from which no socio-economic characteristics could be deduced. Each version presented the same disease underlying the signs and symptoms presented. We failed to find any meaningful differences in diagnostic accuracy and time-to-diagnosis.

Our experiment presents a third attempt to experimentally study the influence of socio-economic status on the precision of physicians’ diagnoses and their engagement with patients of different socio-economic backgrounds, expressed as the amount of time they spent on diagnosing the cases. Two previous attempts, also failing to find differences [15, 16], had serious methodological shortcomings, and our design remediated these. The common assumption was, that doctors, perhaps due to implicit prejudices, would attach less value to the health of poorer patients and therefore would provide less appropriate care. Prejudices against poor people are broadly shared [20, 21]. Paul Gorski [22], in his “Myth of the culture of poverty” mentions a number of these preconceptions with regard to poor people: Poor people would be unmotivated and have weak work ethics; poor parents are supposed to be uninvolved in their children’s learning, largely because they do not value education; and poor people tend to abuse drugs and alcohol (all turn out to be largely false). Our participants did not seem to suffer from those prejudices: they spent equal amounts of time on each of the patients presented, and came to similar diagnostic decisions, irrespective of the socio-economic background of the patient. With some confidence, and in the light of the previous findings, we conclude that, at least within the realm of experimental approaches to the issue, doctors do not treat their patients differently based on whether they are rich or poor. We state this conclusion with some certainty because our study used a within-group design, excluding the possibility of confounding factors due to poor randomization. In addition, we have used the same methodology in similar studies, demonstrating that our procedures are sensitive to experimental manipulations of the kind attempted in this study [19, 23]. For instance, in a study on the negative influence of so called “difficult patients,” using the same methods, we were able to demonstrate rather strong effects with similarly subtle differences in the descriptions of patients [17, 18]. Finally, within-group variances are similar to those of other studies using the same methods.

A problem is the external validity of our findings. An experimental approach forces researchers to reduce complexity of the event studied, so that internal validity is assured. (Internal validity guaranties that causal conclusions drawn from the findings are accurate). However, it is possible that physicians do not so much respond to more or less “objective” indicators of poverty, such as a job that someone has, or the community where he or she comes from, but to more subtle characteristics of people in the consulting room, such as their smell, the way they talk, or the way they interact. Such characteristics cannot be studied with the approach used in this study and the ones summarized here. This implies that there is room for further study.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, et al. Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. Am Psychol. 1994;49(1):15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Meara ER, Richards S, Cutler DM. The gap gets bigger: changes in mortality and life expectancy, by education, 1981–2000. Health Aff. 2008;27(2):350–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Nandi A, Glymour MM, Subramanian S. Association among socioeconomic status, health behaviors, and all-cause mortality in the United States. Epidemiology. 2014;25(2):170–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Starfield B. Effects of poverty on health status. Bull N Y Acad Med. 1992;68(1):17.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Winkleby MA, Jatulis DE, Frank E, Fortmann SP. Socioeconomic status and health: how education, income, and occupation contribute to risk factors for cardiovascular disease. Am J Public Health. 1992;82(6):816–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Baum A, Garofalo J, Yali A. Socioeconomic status and chronic stress: does stress account for SES effects on health? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896(1):131–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Psychosocial resources and the SES-health relationship. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896(1):210–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Vliegenthart J, Noppe G, Van Rossum E, Koper J, Raat H, Van den Akker E. Socioeconomic status in children is associated with hair cortisol levels as a biological measure of chronic stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;65:9–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Darmon N, Drewnowski A. Contribution of food prices and diet cost to socioeconomic disparities in diet quality and health: a systematic review and analysis. Nutr Rev. 2015;73(10):643–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Shortt SE, Shaw RA. Equity in Canadian health care: does socioeconomic status affect waiting times for elective surgery? Can Med Assoc J. 2003;168(4):413–6.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Breen N, Figueroa JB. Stage of breast and cervical cancer diagnosis in disadvantaged neighborhoods: a prevention policy perspective. Am J Prev Med. 1996;12(5):319–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Garb HN. Race bias, social class bias, and gender bias in clinical judgment. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 1997;4(2):99–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Mandelblatt J, Andrews H, Kerner J, Zauber A, Burnett W. Determinants of late stage diagnosis of breast and cervical cancer: the impact of age, race, social class, and hospital type. Am J Public Health. 1991;81(5):646–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Rathore SS, Berger AK, Weinfurt KP, Feinleib M, Oetgen WJ, Gersh BJ, et al. Race, sex, poverty, and the medical treatment of acute myocardial infarction in the elderly. Circulation. 2000;102(6):642–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Arber S, McKinlay J, Adams A, Marceau L, Link C, O’Donnell A. Patient characteristics and inequalities in doctors' diagnostic and management strategies relating to CHD: a video-simulation experiment. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62(1):103–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Mohamed F, Mamede S, Mohamedani M, Alwan IA, Magzoub MEM, Schmidt HG. The effect of patients appearance on doctors’ diagnostic decision making: do poor people get poorer medical care? Health Prof Educ. 2016;2(1):18–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Mamede S, Van Gog T, Schuit SCE, Van den Berge K, Van Daele PLA, Bueving H, et al. Why patients' disruptive behaviours impair diagnostic reasoning: a randomised experiment. BMJ Qual Saf. 2017;26(1):13–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Schmidt HG, van Gog T, Schuit SCE, Van den Berge K, Van Daele PLA, Bueving H, et al. Do patients' disruptive behaviours influence the accuracy of a doctor's diagnosis? A randomised experiment. BMJ Qual Saf. 2017;26(1):19–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Mamede S, van Gog T, van den Berge K, van Saase JLCM, Schmidt HG. Why do doctors make mistakes? A study of the role of salient distracting clinical features. Acad Med. 2014;89(1):114–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Cozzarelli C, Wilkinson AV, Tagler MJ. Attitudes toward the poor and attributions for poverty. J Soc Issues. 2001;57(2):207–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Lott B. Cognitive and behavioral distancing from the poor. Am Psychol. 2002;57(2):100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Gorski P. The myth of the “culture of poverty”. Educ Leadersh. 2008;65(7):32.

    Google Scholar 

  23. ALQahtani DA, Rotgans JI, Mamede S, ALAlwan I, Magzoub MEM, Altayeb FM, et al. Does time pressure have a negative effect on diagnostic accuracy? Acad Med. 2016;91(5):710–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would kike to appreciate the contribution of King Abdulaziz Medical City for funding this study.

Funding

This research is funded by King Abduaziz Medical City, Saudi Arabia Grant Number 84/34. The funding agent has no reole in the design of the study; data collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; and the writing of manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IA help in the design of the study, data collection and manuscript revision, MM help in the writing proposal, study design data collection and manuscript writing, AA helped in data collection and manuscript revision, SM helped in study design, data analysis and manuscript writing, SA helped in data collection and manuscript revision, AB Helped in Manuscript revision and data collection, HS helped in study design, data analysis and manuscript writing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mohi Eldin Magzoub.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This research is approved by the IRB committee of King Abdullah International Medical Recearch Center, Saudi Arabia.

All participants signed consent to use their data.

Consent for publication

Not Applicable

Competing interests

The corresponding author (MM) is the member of the Editorial Board of BMC medical education (Associate editors. Other authors no competing interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Al Alwan, I., Magzoub, M.E., Al Haqwi, A. et al. Do poor patients suffer from inaccurate diagnoses more than well-to-do patients? A randomized control trial. BMC Med Educ 19, 386 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-019-1805-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12909-019-1805-6

Keywords